Career choices are very important decisions in life with many long-term consequences. In recent years, much attention has been paid to what motivates youth to pursue STEM (science, technology, engineering, mathematics) careers, given the high demand in the current labor market. The situated expectancy-value theory (Eccles & Wigfield,
2020; Wigfield & Eccles,
2000) is one of the most well-known theories for explaining how such occupational choices arise. The theory has the following simplified core assumption, which was confirmed by a large number of studies over the last decades (for a summary see Eccles & Wigfield,
2020): Individuals choose to engage in tasks, activities, and domains they expect to do well in (expectancies for success, or more generally competence beliefs, often operationalized via academic self-concepts) and in which they see value (value beliefs). The situated expectancy-value theory assumes additionally that value beliefs are stronger predictors of choices than competence-related variables (Eccles & Wigfield,
2024) and empirical support of this assumption led to the recommendation that value beliefs should be the main target of career choice interventions (for a recent review see Rosenzweig et al.,
2022). However, research on the longer-term causal effects (assessed over more than two time points and with current models) of the two constructs and their indirect effects is missing. This study re-examines the empirical evidence of the special importance of value beliefs for career aspirations and choices in the STEM domain as it is highly relevant for planning interventions.
Situated Expectancy-Value Theory and Open Questions Regarding Interventions
According to situated expectancy-value theory (Eccles & Wigfield,
2024), individuals choose to engage in tasks, activities, and domains in which they expect to do well, and which have a high overall subjective value for them. One’s overall value is assumed to be an aggregate of intrinsic value (a task or domain is enjoyable), attainment value (the importance of a task or domain), utility value (a task or domain is useful (for one’s plans)), and cost (the cost/benefit ratio of a task or domain). It is ingrained in situated expectancy-value theory that these value beliefs are more important for engagement and choices than competence beliefs, while competence beliefs are more important for achievement (Eccles & Wigfield,
2024; Rosenzweig et al.,
2022). However, while the higher predictive strength of value beliefs for career choices is empirically well established in observational studies and interventions typically focus on value beliefs (for a recent review see Rosenzweig et al.,
2022), from a developmental perspective, two related aspects complicate the decision whether to focus more on value beliefs or competence beliefs in interventions, especially when the interventions are conducted a relatively long time before the actual career choices.
First, besides the direct effects of competence beliefs and value beliefs on career aspirations and choices from one time point to the next, situated expectancy-value theory also assumes indirect effects over time. The most prominent one is the indirect effect of competence beliefs on career aspirations and choices via value beliefs. It is assumed that students start to value what they feel competent in as this is intrinsically rewarding, probably primarily due to eliciting positive affect, similar to processes in classical conditioning and positive reinforcement (Eccles,
2009). Additionally, students seem to maintain a positive view of themselves by devaluing tasks or domains in which they feel not competent (Eccles,
2009). Furthermore, an indirect effect from value beliefs on career aspirations and choices via competence beliefs is also expected as increased engagement in the domain, resulting from valuing it, can lead to higher skills and, in consequence, an increase in competence beliefs (Eccles,
2009). Empirical findings indicate that the development of value beliefs indeed depends on competence beliefs as assumed, but much less the other way around (for a summary, see Benden & Lauermann,
2023). This implies an indirect effect of competence beliefs on career aspirations and choices via value beliefs and only a smaller indirect effect from value beliefs via competence beliefs, if at all. Accordingly, value beliefs should mostly have a direct effect on career aspirations and choices, and the longer-term effect of increasing value beliefs at a certain time point should strongly depend on how much the consequent increase in career aspirations carries over to later points in time; technically this is also an indirect effect via earlier levels of career aspirations.
These considerations of indirect effects, which must be interpreted causally to make sense (e.g., MacKinnon & Pirlott,
2015; Rohrer et al.,
2022), lead to the second point complicating the decision of what to focus on in interventions when based on observational data: Prediction is not causation and to evaluate what consequences changes in competence beliefs and value beliefs have, a strong emphasis should be placed on evidence for causality. However, aside from randomized trials, causal evidence is very hard to obtain as it needs special longitudinal designs and analyses to provide at least a certain degree of causal evidence (Murayama & Gfrörer,
2024; Vanderweele et al.,
2020). In the domain of interest, STEM, there are surprisingly few longitudinal studies regarding both the mentioned indirect effects of competence beliefs and value beliefs on career aspirations and choices as well as regarding evidence for causality from observational data. Most previous situated expectancy-value theory studies, which found the discussed dominance of value beliefs, examined competence beliefs and value beliefs at a single point in time simultaneously and predicted outcomes such as STEM career aspirations (e.g., Jansen et al.,
2021) or choices (e.g., Wille et al.,
2020) at a single later point in time, controlling for basic covariates. It is reasonable to assume that only the inclusion of basic covariates is not sufficient to reduce confounding substantially as the outcome at an earlier time point is the most impactful covariate for reducing confounding (Vanderweele et al.,
2020), acting as a confound-blocker (Wysocki et al.,
2022). To be fair, when studies investigate future STEM choices like studying a STEM subject, there is, of course, no identical measure of the outcome at a previous time point to control for. The situation is different, however, in longitudinal studies examining the effects of competence beliefs and value beliefs on career aspirations. In this case the possibility exists to ask repeatedly about career aspirations and include the outcome at a previous time point in the model, which reduces the risk of confounding substantially. Still, there are only two such studies in the context of situated expectancy-value theory assessing competence beliefs, value beliefs, and STEM career aspirations at more than one time point (namely two) and controlling for the previous time point(s) (Lauermann et al.,
2017; Lazarides & Lauermann,
2019). Both studies found the typical effects of competence beliefs (assessed via self-concepts) and value beliefs on career aspirations, but interestingly, these studies did not find stronger effects of value beliefs compared to competence beliefs, indicating that both constructs could be equally important targets for interventions to improve STEM career aspirations.
The above-mentioned studies assessing the main constructs of situated expectancy-value theory at two time points provide some evidence for effects involved in the assumed indirect effects (for example, the causal path from competence beliefs to the mediator value beliefs or the causal path from the mediator value beliefs to the outcome career aspirations). However, two time points are insufficient for properly testing complete mediation chains, for example, from self-concepts to value beliefs to career aspirations. The reason is that the proper temporal order cannot be ensured with only two time points. To investigate whether competence beliefs influence value beliefs and whether value beliefs influence career aspirations would need three time points, the only other option being to make more untestable assumptions (e.g., that effects are stable over time). Modeling over more than two time points allows to investigate how the system develops from state to state and if the effects between the variables are stable over time. Additionally, it allows to estimate total effects over the whole study period (Cole & Maxwell,
2003). These effects consist of all possible paths between two variables over the study period.
Accordingly, conducting studies with the relevant situated expectancy-value theory variables assessed at more than two time points and conducting a traditional longitudinal mediation analysis using the standard cross-lagged panel model (CLPM; Cole & Maxwell,
2003) would have advantages, but other problems remain. For example, in the very likely scenario that there are unmeasured confounding variables that affect both the outcome and the predictor directly (for example, rather time-invariant variables such as the socio-economic background), the results of traditional CLPMs become biased (see Murayama & Gfrörer (
2024) for a detailed description which (other) biases can arise with the CLPM). For cross-lagged effects, such bias may sometimes be small enough not to distort results too much (Lüdtke & Robitzsch,
2023). However, for autoregressive effects, the bias can always be expected to be large as it is not possible for the outcome at a previous time point to be a confound blocker for itself. This means many variables could confound the autoregressive effect between the outcome and the outcome at a previous time point in the CLPM. Therefore, the autoregressive effect depends much more than the cross-lagged effect on additional covariates to reduce confounding. As it can be argued that the covariates assessed in typical studies are very often clearly not sufficient, CLPMs are usually not well suited for longitudinal mediation analyses, especially due to biased autoregressive effects. Fortunately, recent extensions of the CLPM like the random-intercept cross-lagged panel model (RI-CLPM; Hamaker et al.,
2015) and the predetermined dynamic panel model (DPM; Allison et al.,
2017; Andersen,
2022), can (better) account for unobserved confounding variables by including latent variables (Usami et al.,
2019). These methods improve causal estimates for cross-lagged and autoregressive effects in many realistic scenarios and should be a clear improvement, especially for longitudinal mediation analyses. However, while including latent variables probably reduces the confounding of autoregressive effects substantially, it should be kept in mind that it still is an approximation that cannot substitute an analysis in which all relevant confounders are assessed directly.