Skip to main content

Welkom bij Scalda & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Scalda heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen.Je kunt de producten hieronder links aanschaffen en rechts inloggen.

Registreer

Schaf de BSL Academy aan: 

BSL Academy mbo AG

Eenmaal aangeschaft kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met Jan van der Velden.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 15-10-2014 | Original Article

Impaired Facial Affect Perception in Unaffected Children at Familial Risk for Panic Disorder

Auteurs: Cynthia Bilodeau, Jacques Bradwejn, Diana Koszycki

Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development | Uitgave 5/2015

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Recent studies suggest that impaired processing of facial affect has a familial component and may reflect a marker of liability to psychopathology. This study investigated whether facial affect processing is impaired in offspring with parental panic disorder (PD). Psychiatrically healthy children with parental PD (n = 51) and age and sex matched control children with no parental psychopathology (n = 51) completed a standard facial recognition task. High-risk children made more errors recognizing fearful faces than controls and misattributed fear and angry facial affect as surprised. High-risk females also made more errors recognizing sad faces compared to low risk females and misattributed sadness as fear. No difference emerged for self-rated anxiety while viewing facial expressions. However, self-rated anxiety correlated moderately with misrecognition of fearful facial affect in high-risk children. Overall, our data suggest that the ability to correctly recognize negative facial emotions is impaired in children with parental PD. Further research is needed to confirm if these deficits represent a trait marker of liability for PD and elucidate the contribution of genetic and family environmental influences.

Introduction

The ability to accurately decode facial emotions is important for adaptive social behaviors, emotional development, and well-being [1, 2]. Recognition of facial expressions begins in infancy and by middle childhood humans are capable of recognizing six basic facial expressions of emotion: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise [2, 3]. Developmental studies indicate that accuracy and speed in recognizing facial affect improve from childhood through early adulthood, plausibly due to maturation of brain structures associated with facial emotion processing and social experiential factors [35]. Individual differences in the ability to detect facial emotions accurately have been documented, especially for complex emotions such as fear and anger. Twin studies suggest that genetic and environmental influences likely account for individual differences in processing facial affect [68]. Although the role of specific genes and mechanisms are largely unknown, available molecular genetic data has linked facial affect processing with genes encoding neurotransmitter receptors, such catecholamine and serotonin receptors [913].
Impaired facial emotion processing has been observed in children and adolescents with a wide range of psychological problems including anxiety disorders [14, 15]. Studies using facial identification tasks report that anxiety disordered youth are less accurate in recognizing facial expressions than non-anxious controls. Specifically, anxious youth make more errors in recognizing negative valence faces [1620] or mislabel positive or neutral emotions [16, 21]. Similarly, children with high trait anxiety have been found to make more errors recognizing negative valence faces than children with low trait anxiety [22]. Neuroimaging findings suggest that these deficits may be attributed to dysregulation of corticolimbic brain circuits [2327], which are most often associated with the evaluation of facial expressions [28, 29]. Other studies, however, report that affect identification sensitivity is intact and unimpaired in anxious youth [3035]. Inconsistent findings are likely attributed to differences in study population, sample size, medication use, and test design including range and intensity of facial stimuli used and exposure duration.
Little is currently known about whether impaired facial affect processing is a premorbid trait marker of anxiety disorder risk or a consequence of anxiety. Preliminary research with individuals at genetic risk for diverse psychiatric disorders suggests these deficits reflect a marker of liability to psychopathology. For example, unaffected first-degree relatives of probands with bipolar disorder [36], schizophrenia [37], and depression [38] have been found to exhibit deficits in labelling facial expressions compared to low-risk controls. Research on facial affect processing in relatives of probands with anxiety disorders is sparse. Pine et al. [39] investigated sensitivity and attention allocation to face photographs in offspring with and without parental panic disorder (PD) and found high-risk children to report more fear and longer latency to report fear during presentations of negative valence facial expressions than low-risk controls. However, as roughly half of the high-risk children had an anxiety disorder, it is unclear to what extent this sensitivity reflects a state or trait marker of anxiety. Nevertheless, this study does suggest that facial affect processing might be a promising mechanism for understanding the reported link between parental PD and elevated risk for the disorder in offspring [40].
The aim of the present study was to further explore facial affect processing in offspring with parental PD. We investigated psychiatrically healthy children in order to avoid the confounding effects of child psychopathology on outcome. Based on findings that patients with PD exhibit deficits in recognizing negative valence facial affect [4143], especially threat-related expressions (i.e., anger and fear) which are postulated to signal threat in the environment via activation of the amygdala [29], we examined whether high-risk offspring would also exhibit deficits in recognizing threat-related emotions relative to low-risk controls. Further, based on findings by Pine and colleagues [39], we determined whether high-risk offspring would also report higher levels of subjective anxiety while viewing negative facial expressions of emotions than the control children. As sex can influence the accuracy of emotion recognition [3] and is a risk factor for PD [44], we also explored the interaction between risk group and sex on response to the facial recognition task. Additionally, the study controlled for the effect of anxious traits as they too may moderate response to facial affect processing tasks [22, 45].

Materials and methods

Participants

Facial recognition data were collected from children who participated in two separate studies on biological and psychological risk factors predisposing to anxiety. Children with and without parental PD were recruited through advertisements in local newspapers and the Internet and through posters in hospitals, family medicine centers and universities. To be eligible for the studies families had to have one or more biological child between the ages of 7–18 years with no documented history of psychiatric disorders. Children were excluded if they had a history of brain injury, clinically significant and/or unstable medical conditions, or used medications with peripheral or central nervous system effects. High-risk (HR) families had to have a parent with a current or past history of primary PD with or without agoraphobia (PD ± AG). For low-risk (LF) families neither parent could have a history of psychopathology. The institutional review boards approved the studies (REB #2008006 and #H- 09-09-08) and written informed consent was obtained from the child’s legal guardian as well as the child’s assent. Offspring 16 years and over provided their own consent. Families were compensated for their participation in the study.

Assessment Procedures

Parents who expressed an interest in the research completed an initial telephone pre-screen with a research assistant who explained the purpose of the study and obtained information about the psychiatric status of both biological parents, as well as history of psychiatric symptoms, medical illness and medication use in their offspring. If the family was potentially eligible, a second telephone pre-screen was scheduled to confirm the diagnostic status of the parent. The DSM-IV based structured clinical interview (SCID) [46] was used to assess current or lifetime diagnosis of primary PD ± AG in the parent(s) of HR offspring and the absence of psychopathology in both parents of LR offspring. A registered psychologist conducted interviews with the affected parent and trained research assistants conducted interviews with the control parents.
Potentially eligible children were invited to the research laboratory for a face-to-face screen interview to confirm the absence of a current or past history of any Axis I disorder and other eligibility criteria. The child version of the SCID [47] was used to evaluate children who participated in study 1 and the child version of the Anxiety Disorders Interview [48] was used for children who participated in study 2. Interviewers were clinicians and trained research assistants who were unaware of parental diagnosis. Eligible offspring completed self-report questionnaires and returned to the laboratory for a second visit to complete the facial recognition task and other study procedures. Six children (one HR and five LR) were excluded from the study following the face-to-face assessment and three children (two HR and one LR) who were eligible to participate did not return for the second visit.

Measures

The Pictures of Facial Affect (POFA) [1]

The POFA was used for facial emotions identification. The POFA includes 110 black and white photographs of 8 females and 6 males expressing either an emotion (happiness, sadness, fear, anger, disgust, or surprise) or a neutral facial expression. In the present study, participants were presented with pictures displaying 5 emotions (happiness, sadness, anger, fear, and surprise) or a neutral expression. We did not include disgust because it is the least accurately identified facial affect in children, with rates of accuracy averaging between 30 and 40 % [3]. There were 36 trials divided into 6 blocks of 6 pictures each (3 males, 3 females). Each block included pictures of all 5 facial affect and a neutral expression. Pictures were chosen at random and the presentation order was counterbalanced for each block. Pictures were presented sequentially on a computer screen using PowerPoint presentation. Children were provided with a list of all possible answers and after each picture had been displayed for 10 s, they were instructed to enter the correct choice by pressing a key on the computer keyboard. In the first 5 blocks children labelled the facial expressions. On the 6th block faces were presented in the same way as the others, however, participants were instructed to rate how anxious they felt while viewing each picture on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely).

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children-Trait (STAIC-T) [49]

The STAIC-T is a well established self-report measure of trait anxiety in children. The scale is composed of 20-items rated on a 3-point scale, with higher scores indicating higher levels of trait anxiety. The STAIC-T correlates with childhood measures of anxiety and discriminates between children having high and low tendency to experience anxious states [50]. Retest reliability values range from 0.65 to 0.71 and the scale has satisfactory internal consistency, with an alpha coefficient of 0.80 [49]. In the current sample Cronbach’s alpha was 0.90.

Children Anxiety Sensitivity Index (CASI) [51]

The CASI is a self-report measure that assesses beliefs children have about the negative consequences of anxiety. It includes 18 items that are rated on a 3-point Likert scale. The CASI correlates with childhood measures of fear and anxiety [52, 53], discriminates between children with and without anxiety disorders [53], and predicts onset of spontaneous panic [54]. Psychometric evaluation of the CASI in children ages 6–17 years reveal internal consistency estimates of 0.87 and test–retest reliability estimates of 0.76 and 0.79 for nonclinical and clinical samples, respectively [51]. In the current sample Cronbach’s alpha was 0.83.

Child Self Report of Current Inhibition (CSRCI) [55]

The CSRCI is a 30-item self-report measure of childhood behavioral inhibition (BI) and is written in a language appropriate for children as young as seven years. The scale assesses behaviours related to inhibition including separation anxiety, social withdrawal, fears, and illness complaints, which are rated on a 5-point Likert scale. The content of the items on the CSRI directly parallel those of its adult equivalent, the Retrospective Childhood Inhibition Scale [55], from which the CSRI was derived. Although published data on the psychometric properties of the CSRI are currently unavailable, research on the RCIS indicates high internal consistency (αs range from 0.79 to 0.91). In the current sample Cronbach’s alpha was 0.80.

Data Analysis

Data were analyzed with SPSS Version 21.0. Due to a problem with the computer keyboard data from 2.9 % of trials was missing. Little’s Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) test revealed no systematic pattern of missing data across the trials. Missing data for the number of errors committed and VAS anxiety ratings were imputed with the Expectation Maximization procedure. This imputation procedure is an acceptable practice for dealing with data missing completely at random. All available data were used for analysis of error patterns.
Mixed model analysis was used to determine the association between risk group and the number of errors committed in identifying faces, error patterns, and self-rated anxiety while viewing facial stimuli. The risk group × sex interaction was evaluated in the model and significant interactions resulted in subsequent stratified analysis. Family was included as a random effect in the model because some families included more than one child. A priori covariates included the STAIC-T, CASI and CSRCI. Age and ethnicity were also included as covariates in the model due to the wide age range of our sample and unexpected group difference in the proportion of Caucasian versus non-Caucasian children. Mean errors in recognizing happy, sad, surprised, mad, and neutral faces and VAS anxiety ratings while viewing happy, neutral, mad, and scared faces were skewed and analysis was performed on both untransformed and logarithmically transformed data. However, as results were comparable for both analyses untransformed results are reported herein. Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) and the 95 % confidence intervals (95 % CI) around the effect sizes were calculated to examine the magnitude of risk group differences. Finally, Pearson’s correlations were computed to assess the relationship between covariates and dependent measures and between errors in decoding facial affect and self-rated anxiety. Significance was set at 0.05 for all analyses. Due to the preliminary nature of this research and relatively small sample size we did not control for multiple tests.

Results

Sample Characteristics

Facial recognition data were available for 51 HR children from 38 families with parental PD. The affected parent was the mother in 71 % of families and in one family both parents had the disorder. The mean age of PD onset was 28.87 ± 10.5 years and 31 % of parents were in remission. HR children comprised 28 females and 23 males with a mean age of 10.9 ± 2.9 years. Forty-seven children were Caucasian (92 %), one was Asian (2 %), and three were mixed race (6 %). The 51 LR offspring derived from 43 families were matched for age and gender. Thirty-five were Caucasian (69 %), five were Asian (10 %), six were black (12 %), one was Hispanic (2 %), and four were mixed race (8 %). Child ethnicity differed between risk groups, with the HR group including a smaller percentage of non-Caucasian children than the LR group (Chi square = 6.47, df = 1, p = 0.012). Mean (±SD) scores for anxiety-related traits for high and low risk children were, respectively, 1.84 ± 0.4 and 1.81 ± 0.4 for the CSRCI, 26.47 ± 9.5 and 26.18 ± 5.3 for the CASI, and 30.88 ± 7.9 and 29.92 ± 7.0 for the STAIC-T. Differences between groups on mean anxiety-related trait scores were not statistically significant (ps ≥ 0.05). Correlations between covariates and dependent measures for the total sample are shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Correlations between covariates and total number of errors committed and anxiety ratings while viewing facial affect for the total sample
Dependent measures
Age
STAIC-T
CASI
CSRCI
Ethnicity of child
Number of errors committed
     
 Angry faces
−0.03
−0.14
−0.06
0.06
−0.03
 Surprised faces
−0.16
0.05
0.11
0.14
−0.17
 Neutral faces
−0.24*
0.10
0.08
0.08
−0.08
 Sad faces
0.01
−0.01
0.01
0.11
0.12
 Scared faces
−0.19
−0.12
−0.07
−0.03
−0.07
 Happy faces
0.05
0.11
0.02
0.16
0.06
Anxiety while viewing facial affect
     
 Angry faces
−0.18
0.05
0.01
0.21*
−0.06
 Surprised faces
0.08
0.17
0.09
0.2*
0.03
 Neutral faces
−0.17
0.22*
0.14
0.33**
0.12
 Sad faces
0.01
0.18
0.17
0.16
0.002
 Scared faces
−0.22*
−0.10
0.02
−0.03
0.01
 Happy faces
−0.06
0.14
0.15
0.27**
0.03
STAIC-T State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children-Trait, CASI Children Anxiety Sensitivity Index, CSRCI Child Self Report of Current Inhibition
p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01

Emotion Recognition and Processing

Mean errors in facial affect recognition are shown in Table 2. After controlling for age, ethnicity and anxiety-related traits, the analysis revealed a significant effect for risk group in errors committed identifying fearful affect, with HR children committing more errors than LR controls [F(1, 93) = 5.84, p = 0.018, Cohen’s d = 0.43 (95 % CI 0.04–0.82)]. Analysis of error patterns showed that HR children were more likely than LR controls to mislabel fearful facial affect as surprised [mean number fearful affect labelled as surprised: 1.90 ± 1.4 versus 1.28 ± 1.2 for HR and LR groups, respectively, F(1, 88) = 5.14, p = 0.026, Cohen’s d = 0.47 (95 % CI 0.08–0.87)]. A significant group × sex interaction [F(2, 93) = 4.35, p = 0.016] emerged for errors committed identifying sad affect, with HR females committing more errors than LR females [mean (±SD) error score: 1.39 ± 1.0 versus 0.60 ± 0.8, p = 0.002, Cohen’s d = 0.89 (95 % CI 0.32–1.42)]. No differences between high and low risk males were noted. Analysis of error patterns showed that HR females were more likely than LR females to mislabel sad affect as fear [mean (SD) number sad affect labelled as fear 0.48 ± 0.7 versus 0.12 ± 0.3, F(1, 44) = 5.30, p = 0.03, Cohen’s d = 0.70 (95 % CI 0.16–1.24)].
Table 2
Mean (±SD) errors committed identifying facial affect
Facial affect
High risk children
Low risk children
Cohen’s d (95 % CI)
Angry
0.97 ± 0.69
0.84 ± 0.78
0.11 (−0.28 to 0.50)
Surprised
0.47 ± 0.70
0.46 ± 0.65
0.01 (−0.37 to 0.40)
Neutral
1.03 ± 1.54
0.76 ± 0.81
0.22 (−0.17 to 0.61)
Sad
1.22 ± 1.02
0.95 ± 1.02
0.26 (−0.12 to 0.65)
Fearful
2.15 ± 1.47
1.57 ± 1.22
0.43 (0.04 to 0.82)
Happy
0.25 ± 0.52
0.24 ± 0.51
0.02 (−0.37 to 0.40)
Values are unadjusted means. CI confidence intervals
The number of errors committed identifying neutral, happy, surprised, and angry faces was comparable for high- and low-risk children (ps ranged from 0.28 to 0.84 for risk group main effects and from 0.15 to 0.75 for risk group × sex interactions). When the error pattern for these faces was examined, HR children were more likely than LR controls to mislabel angry faces as surprised (mean (SD) number of angry faces labelled as surprised 0.57 ± 0.5 versus 0.33 ± 0.5 [F(1, 86) = 8.25, p = 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.49 (95 % CI 0.10–0.89)].
Mean anxiety ratings while viewing each affect are shown in Table 3. A significant group × sex interaction emerged for fear while viewing angry faces [F(2, 93) = 3.79, p = 0.03], however none of the pairwise comparisons were statistically significant. No effect of group or group × sex interaction was detected for the other anxiety ratings (ps ranged from 0.17 to 0.90 for risk group main effects and from 0.16 to 0.96 for risk group × sex interactions). Correlation analysis revealed that among HR children, a significant positive association emerged between anxiety ratings and the number of errors committed identifying fearful (r = 0.37, p = 0.008) and neutral (r = 0.30, p = 0.03) affect. Among LR children, a significant positive correlation emerged between anxiety ratings and the number of errors committed identifying neutral face (r = 0.34, p = 0.02). Differences between correlation coefficients between high- and low-risk children were not statistically significant (ps ranged from 0.37 to 0.88).
Table 3
Mean (±SD) anxiety ratings while viewing facial affect by risk group
Facial affect
High risk children
Low risk children
Cohen’s d (95 % CI)
Angry
1.56 ± 0.89
1.46 ± 0.82
0.10 (−0.08 to 0.49)
Surprised
2.67 ± 1.28
2.78 ± 1.24
−0.15 (−0.54 to 0.23)
Neutral
1.94 ± 1.07
2.00 ± 1.00
−0.14 (−0.52 to 0.25)
Sad
2.46 ± 1.05
2.40 ± 1.17
0.02 (−0.37 to 0.41)
Fearful
1.34 ± 0.88
1.65 ± 1.15
−0.34 (−0.73 to 0.05)
Happy
1.90 ± 0.91
2.04 ± 1.10
−0.17 (−0.56 to 0.21)
Values are unadjusted means. CI confidence intervals

Discussion

To our knowledge this is the first study to assess facial affect recognition in unaffected offspring with parental PD. The results indicate that HR children exhibit deficits in recognizing specific negative valence emotions, whereas recognition of positive and neutral affect appears to be intact. As predicted, HR children made more errors decoding fearful faces than LR controls. This finding is consistent with studies demonstrating that anxious youth [20] and adult patients with PD [4143] exhibit deficits in recognizing threat-related facial expressions such as anger and fear. Further analysis indicated that HR children mislabeled angry and fearful faces as surprised, although the significance of this finding is unclear as surprised is an ambiguous facial expression that allows for either positive or negative interpretations [56]. However, recent research by Tottenham et al. [57] revealed that children and adolescents typically show a negative bias when viewing surprised faces compared to adults, and that confusion between angry, fearful, and surprised faces in youth is likely due to a negative interpretation of surprised faces rather than perceptual similarity. This would suggest that our HR children exhibited a negative bias when they mislabeled anger and fear as surprised, although we cannot exclude the possibility that these emotions were also misperceived as a positive valence affect.
Our data also showed a significant difference between PD risk groups in the capacity to discriminate sad faces based on gender. Specifically, HR females made more errors in identifying sad affect than LR females and misperceived sadness as fear. Adult PD patients have also been reported to have lower accuracy in recognizing sad affect and this pattern of error correlates with severity of depressive symptoms [41]. This finding is not surprising as many of the brain structures involved in the processing of sad and fearful facial affect overlap [58]. Individuals at risk for depression are faster at recognizing fearful faces than LR controls [59] and brain imaging studies have found youth at familial risk for depression to show greater amygdala activation in response to fearful facial expressions than LR controls [15]. Children with a history of major depression show perturbed encoding of fearful faces [60] and depressed girls exhibit a blunted amygdala response to fearful faces compared anxious and healthy control girls [23]. A blunted amygdala response to fearful faces has also been observed in medicated adults with PD and is thought to reflect a compensatory response to amygdala hyperreactivity to threat-related stimuli [61]. Considering that panic attacks, PD and depression are highly comorbid [62], it is possible that deficits in recognizing sad facial affect is a vulnerability marker of risk for depression in females with parental PD. This question merits further empirical research.
We could not confirm our hypothesis that HR children would also experience more subjective anxiety while viewing facial affects compared to control children. Although Pine et al. [39] reported that offspring at risk for PD experienced more fear while viewing evocative facial affect than LR offspring, their HR sample included symptomatic children which may have confounded findings. Unlike Pine et al.’s study we did not measure reaction time for rating facial affect and cannot exclude the possibility that our unaffected HR children would have exhibited a slower reaction time for rating negative valence emotions than controls. We also measured subjective anxiety in the final block of face presentations and differences between risk groups may have emerged if we obtained anxiety ratings during additional blocks of face presentations. Nevertheless, in contrast to LR children, HR children exhibited a moderate and significant positive correlation between self-report anxiety and errors recognizing fearful affect, suggesting that viewing this emotion evoked heightened emotional reactivity in these children.
The facial recognition task is purported to provide a window into the functioning of the amygdala [28, 39]. Dysregulation of the amygdala has been implicated in both the pathophysiology of PD [63] and impaired face-emotion processing in anxious and anxiety-prone individuals [16, 2327, 64]. We are unaware of any published neuroimaging studies of facial affect recognition in unaffected offspring at familial risk for panic or other anxiety disorders. This type of research is needed to map brain networks involved in the processing of facial affect in these vulnerable individuals. Prospective neuroimaging studies are also needed to investigate whether face processing brain networks in unaffected at-risk offspring are stable or change over time due to maturation or other processes, such as exposure to negative life events which often precipitate onset of panic attacks [65, 66] or the emergence of subsyndromal symptoms of pathological anxiety. Such research holds promise to advance our understanding of the trajectory of PD risk and course of illness, and elucidate whether alterations in brain networks presumed to be involved in facial affect processing are a general trait marker of risk or secondary to anxiety states.
Overall, our data concur with reports that deficits in emotion recognition may have a familial component [3638]. PD aggregates in families [40, 67] with heritability estimated at 40 % [67]. Although many genetic polymorphisms have been tested in association studies the genetic basis for PD is largely unknown [68]. A more recent focus in genetic research of PD has been to discover heritable trait markers of disorder vulnerability. Our preliminary data suggest that deficits in processing fearful and sad affect may be a potential trait marker, although more research is needed to replicate findings and establish if these deficits meet criteria for an endophenotype. From a genetic perspective, there is supportive evidence the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTT) may play a role in the processing of fearful facial affect. Young children with the short allele of the 5-HTT gene have been reported to be less accurate in recognizing fearful faces but not other facial affect than children with the long allele [69]. In a study of undergraduate students, short allele carriers of the 5-HTT gene were more accurate in recognizing fearful affect but less accurate in recognizing happy affect than long allele carriers [10]. The differential impact of the 5-HTT gene on recognizing fearful faces in children versus young adults possibly reflects developmental changes whereby deficits transition from poor identification to hypervigilance. Several imaging genetic studies have also shown that variation of the 5-HTT gene confers heightened amygdala reactivity to threatening facial emotions [7072]. It would be worthwhile for future research to determine if the 5-HTT gene and other candidate genes associated with PD risk confer deficits in facial affect recognition in unaffected offspring with parental PD.
In addition to genetic influences it is important to consider the influence of family environment in shaping children’s emotion recognition skills. There is emerging evidence that parenting style and parent–child attachment can influence a child’s ability to accurately recognize and interpret emotional cues [7375]. As parents with PD have been reported in some studies to have a rejecting and overprotective parenting style [76, 77], it is plausible that negative parenting mediates the relationship between parental PD and impaired facial affect recognition in offspring. Parental displays of negative emotions, especially anger and criticism, have also been associated with impaired processing of facial affect in children [11, 78, 79]. While no study to date has examined the impact of parental expressions of anxiety on children’s facial affect perception skills, parental modeling of anxious behaviour and cognitions has been reported to have discernable effects on children’s perception of threat [80, 81]. In addition to parental displays of emotions, parental masking of emotions can potentially influence how children learn to recognize emotional faces. Parents who believe emotions are dangerous tend to mask emotional expressions, perhaps in an attempt to shield their child from observing negative emotionality [82]. As patients with PD have negative beliefs about the consequences of anxiety, it is possible that some parents with the disorder suppress emotional expressions of fear in the presence of their children. Although the precise mechanisms are unknown, parental masking of fear could influence how offspring process threat-related facial stimuli. Considering that parents play a pivotal role in their child’s emotional socialization, additional research is needed to clarify the role of family environment on facial affect perception in children with parental PD.
While this study has significant strengths related to the recruitment of psychological healthy offspring of parents with SCID confirmed PD, limitations should be acknowledged. First, the sample size is relatively small and we may not have had sufficient power to detect other differences in facial affect recognition or anxiety ratings while viewing negative valence faces. Second, families were recruited via advertisement and therefore self-selected themselves into the study, limiting generalizability of findings. Third, this study is cross-sectional and we cannot confirm that deficits in facial recognition predict risk for onset of pathological anxiety. Longitudinal studies are needed to determine whether deficits are more common in HR offspring who eventually develop symptoms of anxiety versus those who do not. Fourth, while failure to identify facial expressions correctly is believed to serve as a risk marker for social maladjustment, we did not examine whether performance on the facial recognition task correlates with impaired social interactions. Given that onset of PD has been linked with interpersonal stressors [83] and that many anxiety disordered patients report relational difficulties [84], further work should investigate whether impaired performance on facial cognitive tasks in HR children correlates with problematic processing of social cues in real life social interactions. Fifth, the age range of our sample was relatively broad and it is possible that developmental differences in face-emotion processing influenced outcome. However, as the correlation coefficients between age and number of facial recognition errors were small, it is unlikely that age was a significant predictor of response to the facial recognition task. Another study limitation is that our HR group comprised predominately Caucasian children and high- and low-risk children were not matched on ethnicity. A more ethnically diverse sample may have yielded different results. Finally, we used standard pictures of facial affect of Caucasian posers. It is possible that low intensity emotional expressions, morphed facial expressions and faces from different ethnicities would reveal more subtle differences between risk groups.

Summary

To summarize, the main finding of this study is that psychiatrically healthy children with parental PD exhibit deficits in recognizing negative valence facial emotions. Whether these deficits play a pathogenic role in the development of anxiety in at-risk children remains to be determined in prospective longitudinal research. Although our findings are preliminary and require replication in a larger sample, results could have implications for the development of preventive interventions. Recent work with autistic [85] and depressed [86] children suggests that facial emotion recognition training improves social cognition. Similar findings have been reported in adults with severe psychiatric disorders [87] and elevated levels of depressive symptoms [88]. Preventive interventions that incorporate strategies to reduce biased processing of facial signals that can lead to inappropriate social responses and interpersonal difficulties may also be of benefit to children at risk for panic and other anxiety disorders.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by the Canadian Institute of Health Research awarded to Dr. Diana Koszycki (MOP 68854 and MOP 86556). Dr. Bilodeau’s postdoctoral fellowship was funded by CIHR Grant MOP 86556. We would like to thank Alexandra Kirvan, Christine Ma, Jennifer Knudson, Nina Hedayati and Julie Wallis for research assistance.

Conflict of interest

None.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Ekman P, Friesen W (1976) Pictures of facial affect. Consulting Psychological Press, Palo Alto Ekman P, Friesen W (1976) Pictures of facial affect. Consulting Psychological Press, Palo Alto
2.
go back to reference Herba C, Phillips M (2004) Annotation: development of facial expression recognition from childhood to adolescence: behavioural and neurological perspectives. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:1185–1198CrossRefPubMed Herba C, Phillips M (2004) Annotation: development of facial expression recognition from childhood to adolescence: behavioural and neurological perspectives. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:1185–1198CrossRefPubMed
3.
go back to reference Williams LM, Mathersul D, Palmer DM, Gur RC, Gur RE, Gordon E (2011) Explicit identification and implicit recognition of facial emotions: I. Age effects in males and females across 10 decades. J Clin Exper Neuropsychol 31:257–277CrossRef Williams LM, Mathersul D, Palmer DM, Gur RC, Gur RE, Gordon E (2011) Explicit identification and implicit recognition of facial emotions: I. Age effects in males and females across 10 decades. J Clin Exper Neuropsychol 31:257–277CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Herba C, Landau S, Russell T, Ecker C, Phillips ML (2006) The development of emotion- processing in children: effects of age, emotion, and intensity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47:1098–1106CrossRefPubMed Herba C, Landau S, Russell T, Ecker C, Phillips ML (2006) The development of emotion- processing in children: effects of age, emotion, and intensity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47:1098–1106CrossRefPubMed
5.
go back to reference Vicari S, Snitzer Reilly J, Pasqualetti P, Vizzotto A, Caltagirone C (2000) Recognition of facial expressions of emotions in school-age children: the intersection of perceptual and semantic categories. Acta Paediatr 89:836–845CrossRefPubMed Vicari S, Snitzer Reilly J, Pasqualetti P, Vizzotto A, Caltagirone C (2000) Recognition of facial expressions of emotions in school-age children: the intersection of perceptual and semantic categories. Acta Paediatr 89:836–845CrossRefPubMed
6.
7.
go back to reference Lau JYF, Burt M, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Rijsdijk F, Shiffrin N, Eley TC (2009) Individual differences in children’s facial expression recognition ability: the role of nature and nurture. Dev Neuropsychol 34:37–51PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Lau JYF, Burt M, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Rijsdijk F, Shiffrin N, Eley TC (2009) Individual differences in children’s facial expression recognition ability: the role of nature and nurture. Dev Neuropsychol 34:37–51PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
8.
go back to reference Wilmer JB, Germine L, Chabris CF, Chatterjee G, Williams M, Loken K, Duchaine G (2010) Human face recognition is specific and highly heritable. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107:5238–5241PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Wilmer JB, Germine L, Chabris CF, Chatterjee G, Williams M, Loken K, Duchaine G (2010) Human face recognition is specific and highly heritable. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107:5238–5241PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
9.
go back to reference Beevers CG, Wells TT, Ellis AJ, McGeary JE (2009) Association of the serotonin transporter gene promoter region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism with biased attention for emotional stimulus. J Abnorm Psychol 118:670–681PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Beevers CG, Wells TT, Ellis AJ, McGeary JE (2009) Association of the serotonin transporter gene promoter region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism with biased attention for emotional stimulus. J Abnorm Psychol 118:670–681PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
10.
go back to reference Defrancesco M, Niederstatter H, Parson W et al (2011) Influence of serotonin transporter genotype and catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158 Met polymorphism on recognition of emotional faces. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 17:1014–1020CrossRefPubMed Defrancesco M, Niederstatter H, Parson W et al (2011) Influence of serotonin transporter genotype and catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158 Met polymorphism on recognition of emotional faces. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 17:1014–1020CrossRefPubMed
11.
go back to reference Gibb BE, Johnson AL, Benas JS, Uhrlass DJ, Knopik VS, McGeary JE (2011) Children’s 5-HTTLPR genotype moderates the link between maternal criticism and attentional biases specifically for facial display of anger. Cogn Emot 25:1104–1120PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Gibb BE, Johnson AL, Benas JS, Uhrlass DJ, Knopik VS, McGeary JE (2011) Children’s 5-HTTLPR genotype moderates the link between maternal criticism and attentional biases specifically for facial display of anger. Cogn Emot 25:1104–1120PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
12.
go back to reference Pérez-Edgar K, Barhaim Y, Mcdermott JM, Chronis-Tuscano A, Pine DS, Fox NA (2010) Attention biases to threat and behavioral inhibition in early childhood shape adolescent social withdrawal. Emotion 10:349–357PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Pérez-Edgar K, Barhaim Y, Mcdermott JM, Chronis-Tuscano A, Pine DS, Fox NA (2010) Attention biases to threat and behavioral inhibition in early childhood shape adolescent social withdrawal. Emotion 10:349–357PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
13.
go back to reference Surguladze SA, Radua J, El-Hage W, Gohier B, Sato JR, Kronhaus DM, Proitsi P, Powell J, Phillips ML (2010) Interactions of catechol-O-methyltransferase and serotonin transporter genes modulates effective connectivity in a facial emotion processing circuitry. Transl Psychiatry 2:e70CrossRef Surguladze SA, Radua J, El-Hage W, Gohier B, Sato JR, Kronhaus DM, Proitsi P, Powell J, Phillips ML (2010) Interactions of catechol-O-methyltransferase and serotonin transporter genes modulates effective connectivity in a facial emotion processing circuitry. Transl Psychiatry 2:e70CrossRef
14.
go back to reference Collin L, Bindra J, Raju M, Gillbert G, Minnis H (2013) Facial emotion recognition in child psychiatry: a systematic review. Res Dev Disabil 34:1505–1520CrossRefPubMed Collin L, Bindra J, Raju M, Gillbert G, Minnis H (2013) Facial emotion recognition in child psychiatry: a systematic review. Res Dev Disabil 34:1505–1520CrossRefPubMed
15.
go back to reference Monk CS, Telzer EH, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Mai Z, Louro HMC, Chen G, McClure-Tone EB, Ernst M, Pine DS (2008) Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65:568–576PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Monk CS, Telzer EH, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Mai Z, Louro HMC, Chen G, McClure-Tone EB, Ernst M, Pine DS (2008) Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65:568–576PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
16.
go back to reference Battaglia M, Ogliari A, Zanoni A, Villa F, Citterio A, Binaghi F, Fossati A, Maffei C (2004) Children’s discrimination of expressions of emotions: relationship with indices of social anxiety and shyness. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 43:358–365CrossRefPubMed Battaglia M, Ogliari A, Zanoni A, Villa F, Citterio A, Binaghi F, Fossati A, Maffei C (2004) Children’s discrimination of expressions of emotions: relationship with indices of social anxiety and shyness. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 43:358–365CrossRefPubMed
17.
go back to reference Easter J, McClure EB, Monk CS, Dhanani M, Hodgdon H, Leibenluft E, Charney DS, Pine DS, Ernst M (2005) Emotion recognition deficits in pediatric anxiety disorders: implications for amygdala research. J Child Adolesc Psychopharm 15:563–570CrossRef Easter J, McClure EB, Monk CS, Dhanani M, Hodgdon H, Leibenluft E, Charney DS, Pine DS, Ernst M (2005) Emotion recognition deficits in pediatric anxiety disorders: implications for amygdala research. J Child Adolesc Psychopharm 15:563–570CrossRef
18.
go back to reference Jarros RB, Salum GA, Da Silva CTB, Toazza R, de Abreau Costa M, de Salles JF, Manfro GG (2012) Anxiety disorders in adolescences are associated with impaired facial expression recognition to negative valence. J Psychiatr Res 46:147–151CrossRefPubMed Jarros RB, Salum GA, Da Silva CTB, Toazza R, de Abreau Costa M, de Salles JF, Manfro GG (2012) Anxiety disorders in adolescences are associated with impaired facial expression recognition to negative valence. J Psychiatr Res 46:147–151CrossRefPubMed
19.
go back to reference Simonian SJ, Beidel DC, Turner SM, Berkes JL, Long JH (2001) Recognition of facial affect by children and adolescents diagnosed with social phobia. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 32:137–145CrossRefPubMed Simonian SJ, Beidel DC, Turner SM, Berkes JL, Long JH (2001) Recognition of facial affect by children and adolescents diagnosed with social phobia. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 32:137–145CrossRefPubMed
20.
go back to reference Walker AS, Nowicki S, Jones J, Heimann L (2011) Errors in identifying and expressing emotions in facial expressions, voices and postures unique to social anxiety. J Genet Psychol 172:293–301CrossRefPubMed Walker AS, Nowicki S, Jones J, Heimann L (2011) Errors in identifying and expressing emotions in facial expressions, voices and postures unique to social anxiety. J Genet Psychol 172:293–301CrossRefPubMed
21.
go back to reference Melfsen S, Florin I (2002) Do socially anxious children show deficits in classifying facial expressions of emotions? J Nonverb Behav 26:109–126CrossRef Melfsen S, Florin I (2002) Do socially anxious children show deficits in classifying facial expressions of emotions? J Nonverb Behav 26:109–126CrossRef
22.
go back to reference Richards A, French CC, Nash G, Hadwin JA, Donnelly NA (2007) Comparison of selective attention and facial processing biases in typically developing children who are high and low in self-reported trait anxiety. Dev Psychopathol 19:481–495CrossRefPubMed Richards A, French CC, Nash G, Hadwin JA, Donnelly NA (2007) Comparison of selective attention and facial processing biases in typically developing children who are high and low in self-reported trait anxiety. Dev Psychopathol 19:481–495CrossRefPubMed
23.
go back to reference Thomas KM, Drevets WC, Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Birmaher B, Eccard CH, Axelson D, Whalen PJ, Casey BJ et al (2001) Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58:1057–1063CrossRefPubMed Thomas KM, Drevets WC, Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Birmaher B, Eccard CH, Axelson D, Whalen PJ, Casey BJ et al (2001) Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58:1057–1063CrossRefPubMed
24.
go back to reference Etkin A, Wager TD (2007) Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry 164:1476–1488PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Etkin A, Wager TD (2007) Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry 164:1476–1488PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
25.
go back to reference Evans KC, Wright CI, Wedig MM, Gold AL, Pollack MH, Rauch SL (2008) A functional MRI study of amygdala responses to angry schematic faces in social anxiety disorder. Depress Anxiety 25:496–505CrossRefPubMed Evans KC, Wright CI, Wedig MM, Gold AL, Pollack MH, Rauch SL (2008) A functional MRI study of amygdala responses to angry schematic faces in social anxiety disorder. Depress Anxiety 25:496–505CrossRefPubMed
26.
go back to reference Hahn A, Stein P, Windischberger C, Weissenbacher A, Spindelegger C, Moser E, Kasper S, Lanzenberger R (2011) Reduced resting-state functional connectivity between amygdale and orbitofrontal cortex in social anxiety disorder. NeuroImage 56:881–889CrossRefPubMed Hahn A, Stein P, Windischberger C, Weissenbacher A, Spindelegger C, Moser E, Kasper S, Lanzenberger R (2011) Reduced resting-state functional connectivity between amygdale and orbitofrontal cortex in social anxiety disorder. NeuroImage 56:881–889CrossRefPubMed
27.
go back to reference McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, Parrish JM, Adler A, Blair RJR, Fromm S, Charney DS, Leibenluft E, Ernst M, Pine DS (2007) Abnormal attention modulation of fear circuit function in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64:97–106CrossRefPubMed McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, Parrish JM, Adler A, Blair RJR, Fromm S, Charney DS, Leibenluft E, Ernst M, Pine DS (2007) Abnormal attention modulation of fear circuit function in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64:97–106CrossRefPubMed
28.
go back to reference Adolphs R (2002) Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 1:21–62CrossRefPubMed Adolphs R (2002) Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 1:21–62CrossRefPubMed
29.
go back to reference Todd RM, Anderson AK (2009) Six degress of separation. The amygdala regulates social distance. Nat Neurosci 12:1–3CrossRef Todd RM, Anderson AK (2009) Six degress of separation. The amygdala regulates social distance. Nat Neurosci 12:1–3CrossRef
30.
go back to reference Allen JL, Abbott MJ, Rapee RM, Coltheart M (2006) Ew gross! Recognitions of expressions of disgust by children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav Change 23:239–249CrossRef Allen JL, Abbott MJ, Rapee RM, Coltheart M (2006) Ew gross! Recognitions of expressions of disgust by children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav Change 23:239–249CrossRef
31.
go back to reference Guyer AE, McClure EB, Adler AD, Brothman MA, Rich BA, Kimes AS, Pine DS, Ernst M, Leibenluft E (2007) Specificity of facial expression labelling deficits in childhood psychopathology. J Child Psychol 48:863–871CrossRef Guyer AE, McClure EB, Adler AD, Brothman MA, Rich BA, Kimes AS, Pine DS, Ernst M, Leibenluft E (2007) Specificity of facial expression labelling deficits in childhood psychopathology. J Child Psychol 48:863–871CrossRef
32.
go back to reference Lau JYF, Hilbert K, Goodman R, Gregory AM, Pine DS, Viding EM, Eley TC (2012) Investigating the genetic and environmental basis of biases in threat recognition and avoidance in children with anxiety problems. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 2:12PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Lau JYF, Hilbert K, Goodman R, Gregory AM, Pine DS, Viding EM, Eley TC (2012) Investigating the genetic and environmental basis of biases in threat recognition and avoidance in children with anxiety problems. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 2:12PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
33.
go back to reference Manassis K, Young A (2000) Perception of emotions in anxious and learning disabled children. Depress Anxiety 12:209–216CrossRefPubMed Manassis K, Young A (2000) Perception of emotions in anxious and learning disabled children. Depress Anxiety 12:209–216CrossRefPubMed
34.
go back to reference McClure EB, Pope K, Hoberman AJ, Pine DS, Leibenluft E (2003) Facial expression recognition in adolescents with mood and anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry 160:1172–1174CrossRefPubMed McClure EB, Pope K, Hoberman AJ, Pine DS, Leibenluft E (2003) Facial expression recognition in adolescents with mood and anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry 160:1172–1174CrossRefPubMed
35.
go back to reference Wong N, Beider DC, Sarver DE, Sims V (2012) Facial emotion recognition in children with high functioning autism and children with social phobia. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 43:775–794CrossRefPubMed Wong N, Beider DC, Sarver DE, Sims V (2012) Facial emotion recognition in children with high functioning autism and children with social phobia. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 43:775–794CrossRefPubMed
36.
go back to reference Brotman MA, Guyer AE, Lawson ES, Horsey SE, Rich BA, Kickstein DP, Pine DS, Leibenluft E (2008) Facial emotion labeling deficits in children and adolescents at risk for bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 165:385–389CrossRefPubMed Brotman MA, Guyer AE, Lawson ES, Horsey SE, Rich BA, Kickstein DP, Pine DS, Leibenluft E (2008) Facial emotion labeling deficits in children and adolescents at risk for bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 165:385–389CrossRefPubMed
37.
go back to reference Leppänen JM, Niehaus DJ, Koen L, Du Toit E, Schoeman R, Emsley R (2008) Deficits in facial affect recognition in unaffected siblings of Xhosa schizophrenia patients: evidence for a neurocognitive endophenotype. Schizophr Res 99:270–273CrossRefPubMed Leppänen JM, Niehaus DJ, Koen L, Du Toit E, Schoeman R, Emsley R (2008) Deficits in facial affect recognition in unaffected siblings of Xhosa schizophrenia patients: evidence for a neurocognitive endophenotype. Schizophr Res 99:270–273CrossRefPubMed
38.
go back to reference Lopez-Duran NL, Kuhlman KR, George C, Kovacs M (2013) Facial emotion expression recognition by children at familial risk for depression: high-risk boys are oversensitive to sadness. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54:565–574PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Lopez-Duran NL, Kuhlman KR, George C, Kovacs M (2013) Facial emotion expression recognition by children at familial risk for depression: high-risk boys are oversensitive to sadness. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54:565–574PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
39.
go back to reference Pine DS, Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Moulton JL, Lisse S, Guardino M, Woldehawariat G (2005) Face-emotion processing in offspring at risk for panic disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44:664–672CrossRefPubMed Pine DS, Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Moulton JL, Lisse S, Guardino M, Woldehawariat G (2005) Face-emotion processing in offspring at risk for panic disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44:664–672CrossRefPubMed
40.
go back to reference Biederman J, Farone SV, Hirshfeld-Becker DR, Friedman D, Rosenbaum JF (2001) Patterns of psychopathology and dysfunction in high-risk children of parents with panic disorder and major depression. Am J Psychiatry 158:49–57CrossRefPubMed Biederman J, Farone SV, Hirshfeld-Becker DR, Friedman D, Rosenbaum JF (2001) Patterns of psychopathology and dysfunction in high-risk children of parents with panic disorder and major depression. Am J Psychiatry 158:49–57CrossRefPubMed
41.
go back to reference Kessler H, Roth J, Von Wietersheim J, Deighton RM, Traue HC (2007) Emotion regulation patterns in patients with panic disorder. Depress Anxiety 24:223–226CrossRefPubMed Kessler H, Roth J, Von Wietersheim J, Deighton RM, Traue HC (2007) Emotion regulation patterns in patients with panic disorder. Depress Anxiety 24:223–226CrossRefPubMed
42.
go back to reference Cai L, Chen W, Shen Y, Wang X, Wei L, Zhang Y, Wang W, Chen W (2012) Recognition of facial expression of emotion in panic disorder. Psychopathology 45:294–299CrossRefPubMed Cai L, Chen W, Shen Y, Wang X, Wei L, Zhang Y, Wang W, Chen W (2012) Recognition of facial expression of emotion in panic disorder. Psychopathology 45:294–299CrossRefPubMed
43.
go back to reference Wang SM, Kim Y, Yeon B, Lee H-K, Kewon Y-S, Lee CT, Lee K-U (2013) Symptom severity of panic disorder associated with impairment in emotion processing of threat- related facial expressions. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 67:245–252CrossRefPubMed Wang SM, Kim Y, Yeon B, Lee H-K, Kewon Y-S, Lee CT, Lee K-U (2013) Symptom severity of panic disorder associated with impairment in emotion processing of threat- related facial expressions. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 67:245–252CrossRefPubMed
44.
go back to reference Pérez-Edgar K, Roberson-Nay R, Hardin MG, Poeth K, Guyer AE, Nelson EE, McClure EB, Henderson HA, Fox NA, Pine DS, Ernst M (2007) Attention alters neural responses to evocative faces in behaviorally inhibited adolescents. Neuroimage 35:1538–1546PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Pérez-Edgar K, Roberson-Nay R, Hardin MG, Poeth K, Guyer AE, Nelson EE, McClure EB, Henderson HA, Fox NA, Pine DS, Ernst M (2007) Attention alters neural responses to evocative faces in behaviorally inhibited adolescents. Neuroimage 35:1538–1546PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
45.
go back to reference Pine DS, Cohen P, Gurley D, Brook J, Ma Y (1998) The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:56–64CrossRefPubMed Pine DS, Cohen P, Gurley D, Brook J, Ma Y (1998) The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:56–64CrossRefPubMed
46.
go back to reference First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW (1996) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID). American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW (1996) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID). American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC
47.
go back to reference Hein D, Matzner F, First M, Spitzer R, Williams J, Gibbon M (1998) Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV childhood diagnoses, KID-SCID. Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Medical School, New York Hein D, Matzner F, First M, Spitzer R, Williams J, Gibbon M (1998) Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV childhood diagnoses, KID-SCID. Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University Medical School, New York
48.
go back to reference Silverman WE, Albano AM (1996) The anxiety disorders interview schedule for DSM-IV—child version. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio Silverman WE, Albano AM (1996) The anxiety disorders interview schedule for DSM-IV—child version. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio
49.
go back to reference Speilberger CD (1973) Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory for children. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto Speilberger CD (1973) Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory for children. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto
50.
go back to reference Southham-Gerow MA, Flannery-Schroeder EC, Kendall PC (2003) A psychometric evaluation of the parent report form of the STATE-TRAIT Anxiety Inventory For Children-Trait Version. J Anxiety Disorders 17:427–446CrossRef Southham-Gerow MA, Flannery-Schroeder EC, Kendall PC (2003) A psychometric evaluation of the parent report form of the STATE-TRAIT Anxiety Inventory For Children-Trait Version. J Anxiety Disorders 17:427–446CrossRef
51.
go back to reference Silverman WK, Fleisig W, Rabian B, Peterson RA (1991) Childhood anxiety sensitivity index. J Clin Child Psychol 20:162–168CrossRef Silverman WK, Fleisig W, Rabian B, Peterson RA (1991) Childhood anxiety sensitivity index. J Clin Child Psychol 20:162–168CrossRef
52.
go back to reference Rabian B, Embry L, MacIntyre D (1999) Behavioral validation of the childhood anxiety sensitivity index in children. J Clin Child Psychol 28:105–112CrossRefPubMed Rabian B, Embry L, MacIntyre D (1999) Behavioral validation of the childhood anxiety sensitivity index in children. J Clin Child Psychol 28:105–112CrossRefPubMed
53.
go back to reference Schmidt NB, Zvolensky MJ, Maner JK (2006) Anxiety sensitivity: prospective predictionof panic attacks and Axis I pathology. J Psychiatry Res 40:691–699CrossRef Schmidt NB, Zvolensky MJ, Maner JK (2006) Anxiety sensitivity: prospective predictionof panic attacks and Axis I pathology. J Psychiatry Res 40:691–699CrossRef
54.
go back to reference Calamari JE, Hale LR, Heffelfinger SK, Janeck AS, Lau JJ, Weerts MA, Taglione PA, Schisler RL (2002) Relations between anxiety sensitivity and panic symptoms in nonreferred children and adolescents. J Behav Therapy Exp Psychol 32:117–136CrossRef Calamari JE, Hale LR, Heffelfinger SK, Janeck AS, Lau JJ, Weerts MA, Taglione PA, Schisler RL (2002) Relations between anxiety sensitivity and panic symptoms in nonreferred children and adolescents. J Behav Therapy Exp Psychol 32:117–136CrossRef
55.
go back to reference Reznick JS, Hegeman IM, Kaufman ER, Woods SW, Jacobs M (1992) Retrospective and concurrent self-report of behavioral inhibition and their relation to adult mental health. Dev Psychopathol 4:301–321CrossRef Reznick JS, Hegeman IM, Kaufman ER, Woods SW, Jacobs M (1992) Retrospective and concurrent self-report of behavioral inhibition and their relation to adult mental health. Dev Psychopathol 4:301–321CrossRef
56.
go back to reference Kohler CG, Turner TH, Gur RE, Gur RC (2004) Recognition of facial emotions in neuropsychiatric disorders. CNS Spectr 9:267–274PubMed Kohler CG, Turner TH, Gur RE, Gur RC (2004) Recognition of facial emotions in neuropsychiatric disorders. CNS Spectr 9:267–274PubMed
57.
go back to reference Tottenham M, Phuong J, Flannery J, Gabard-Durnam L, Goff B (2013) A negativity bias for ambiguous facial expression valence during childhood: converging evidence from behavior and facial corrugator muscle responses. Emotion 13:92–103PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Tottenham M, Phuong J, Flannery J, Gabard-Durnam L, Goff B (2013) A negativity bias for ambiguous facial expression valence during childhood: converging evidence from behavior and facial corrugator muscle responses. Emotion 13:92–103PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
58.
go back to reference Cusi MA, Nazarov A, Holshausen K, Macqueen GM, Mckinnon MC (2011) Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci 37:154–169CrossRef Cusi MA, Nazarov A, Holshausen K, Macqueen GM, Mckinnon MC (2011) Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci 37:154–169CrossRef
59.
go back to reference Le Masurier M, Cowan PJ, Harmer CJ (2007) Emotional bias and waking salivary cortisol in relatives of patients with major depression. Psychol Med 37:403–410CrossRefPubMed Le Masurier M, Cowan PJ, Harmer CJ (2007) Emotional bias and waking salivary cortisol in relatives of patients with major depression. Psychol Med 37:403–410CrossRefPubMed
60.
go back to reference Pine DS, Lissek S, Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Moulton JL, Guardino M, Woldehawariat G (2004) Face-memory and emotion: associations with major depression in children and adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:1199–1208CrossRefPubMed Pine DS, Lissek S, Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Moulton JL, Guardino M, Woldehawariat G (2004) Face-memory and emotion: associations with major depression in children and adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:1199–1208CrossRefPubMed
61.
go back to reference Pillay SS, Gruber SA, Rogowska J, Simpson N, Yurgelun-Todd DA (2006) fMRI of fearful facial affect recognition in panic disorder: the cingulate gyrus-amygdala connection. J Affect Dis 94:173–181CrossRefPubMed Pillay SS, Gruber SA, Rogowska J, Simpson N, Yurgelun-Todd DA (2006) fMRI of fearful facial affect recognition in panic disorder: the cingulate gyrus-amygdala connection. J Affect Dis 94:173–181CrossRefPubMed
62.
go back to reference Roy-Byrne PP, Stang P, Wittchen H-U, Ustun B, Walters E, Kessler RC (2000) Lifetime panic-depression comorbidity in the National Comorbidity Survey: association with symptoms, impairment, course and help seeking. Br J Psychiatry 176:229–235CrossRefPubMed Roy-Byrne PP, Stang P, Wittchen H-U, Ustun B, Walters E, Kessler RC (2000) Lifetime panic-depression comorbidity in the National Comorbidity Survey: association with symptoms, impairment, course and help seeking. Br J Psychiatry 176:229–235CrossRefPubMed
63.
go back to reference Gorman JM, Kent JM, Sullivan GM, Coplan JD (2000) Neuroanatomical hypothesis of panic disorder, revised. Am J Psychiatry 157:493–505CrossRefPubMed Gorman JM, Kent JM, Sullivan GM, Coplan JD (2000) Neuroanatomical hypothesis of panic disorder, revised. Am J Psychiatry 157:493–505CrossRefPubMed
64.
go back to reference Chechko N, Wehrle R, Erdhardt A, Holsboer F, Czisch M, Sämann PG (2009) Unstable prefrontal response to emotional conflict and activation of lower limbic structures and brainstem in remitted panic disorder. PLoS One 4:e5537PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Chechko N, Wehrle R, Erdhardt A, Holsboer F, Czisch M, Sämann PG (2009) Unstable prefrontal response to emotional conflict and activation of lower limbic structures and brainstem in remitted panic disorder. PLoS One 4:e5537PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
65.
go back to reference Klauke B, Deckert J, Reif A, Pauli P, Domschke K (2010) Life events in panic disorder-an update on “candidate stressors”. Depress Anxiety 27:716–730PubMed Klauke B, Deckert J, Reif A, Pauli P, Domschke K (2010) Life events in panic disorder-an update on “candidate stressors”. Depress Anxiety 27:716–730PubMed
66.
go back to reference Scocco P, Barbieri I, Frank E (2007) Interpersonal problems areas and onset of panic disorder. Psychopathology 40:8–13CrossRefPubMed Scocco P, Barbieri I, Frank E (2007) Interpersonal problems areas and onset of panic disorder. Psychopathology 40:8–13CrossRefPubMed
67.
go back to reference Hettema JM, Neale MC, Kendler KS (2001) A review and meta-analysis of the genetic epidemiology of anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry 158:568–578CrossRef Hettema JM, Neale MC, Kendler KS (2001) A review and meta-analysis of the genetic epidemiology of anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry 158:568–578CrossRef
68.
go back to reference Maron E, Hettema JM, Shlik J (2010) Advances in molecular genetics of panic disorder. Mol Psychiatry 15:681–701CrossRefPubMed Maron E, Hettema JM, Shlik J (2010) Advances in molecular genetics of panic disorder. Mol Psychiatry 15:681–701CrossRefPubMed
69.
go back to reference Székely E, Herba CM, Arp PP, Uitterlinden AG, Jaddoe VWV, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, Hudziak JJ, Tiemeier H (2011) Recognition of scared faces and the serotonin transporter gene in young children: the Generation R Study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:1279–1286CrossRefPubMed Székely E, Herba CM, Arp PP, Uitterlinden AG, Jaddoe VWV, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, Hudziak JJ, Tiemeier H (2011) Recognition of scared faces and the serotonin transporter gene in young children: the Generation R Study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:1279–1286CrossRefPubMed
70.
go back to reference Bertolino A, Arciero G, Rubino V, Latorre V, De Candia M, Maxxola V, Blasi G, Caforio G, Hariri A, Kolachana B, Nardini M, Weinberger DR, Scarabino T (2005) Variation of human amygdala response during threatening stimuli as a function of 5′HTTLPR genotype and personality style. Biol Psychiatry 57:1517–1525CrossRefPubMed Bertolino A, Arciero G, Rubino V, Latorre V, De Candia M, Maxxola V, Blasi G, Caforio G, Hariri A, Kolachana B, Nardini M, Weinberger DR, Scarabino T (2005) Variation of human amygdala response during threatening stimuli as a function of 5′HTTLPR genotype and personality style. Biol Psychiatry 57:1517–1525CrossRefPubMed
71.
go back to reference Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Kolachana B, Fera F, Goldman D, Egan MF, Weinberger DR (2002) Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science 297:400–403CrossRefPubMed Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Kolachana B, Fera F, Goldman D, Egan MF, Weinberger DR (2002) Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science 297:400–403CrossRefPubMed
72.
go back to reference Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, Kolachana LS, Mattay VS, Egain MF, Weinberger DR (2005) A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:146–152CrossRefPubMed Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, Kolachana LS, Mattay VS, Egain MF, Weinberger DR (2005) A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:146–152CrossRefPubMed
73.
go back to reference Kujawa A, Dougherty L, Durbin CE, Laptook R, Torpey D, Klein DN (2014) Emotion recognition in preschool children: associations with maternal depression and early parenting. Dev Psychopathol 26:159–170PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Kujawa A, Dougherty L, Durbin CE, Laptook R, Torpey D, Klein DN (2014) Emotion recognition in preschool children: associations with maternal depression and early parenting. Dev Psychopathol 26:159–170PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
74.
go back to reference Zheng L, Chai H, Chen W, Yu R, He W, Jiang Z, Yu S, Li H, Wang W (2011) Recognition of facial emotion and perceived parental bonding styles in healthy volunteers and personality disorder patients. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 65:648–654CrossRefPubMed Zheng L, Chai H, Chen W, Yu R, He W, Jiang Z, Yu S, Li H, Wang W (2011) Recognition of facial emotion and perceived parental bonding styles in healthy volunteers and personality disorder patients. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 65:648–654CrossRefPubMed
75.
go back to reference Steele H, Miriam Steele M, Croft C (2008) Early attachment predicts emotion recognition at 6 and 11 years old. Attach Hum Dev 10:379–393CrossRefPubMed Steele H, Miriam Steele M, Croft C (2008) Early attachment predicts emotion recognition at 6 and 11 years old. Attach Hum Dev 10:379–393CrossRefPubMed
76.
go back to reference Challacombe F, Salkovskis P (2009) A preliminary investigation of the impact of maternal obsessive-compulsive disorder and panic disorder on parenting children. J Anxiety Disord 23:848–857CrossRefPubMed Challacombe F, Salkovskis P (2009) A preliminary investigation of the impact of maternal obsessive-compulsive disorder and panic disorder on parenting children. J Anxiety Disord 23:848–857CrossRefPubMed
77.
go back to reference Schnieder S, Houweling JE, Gommlich-Schneider S, Klein C, Nudel B, Wolke D (2009) Effects of maternal panic disorder on mother-child interactions and relation to child anxiety and child self-efficacy. Arch Womens Ment Health 12:251–259CrossRef Schnieder S, Houweling JE, Gommlich-Schneider S, Klein C, Nudel B, Wolke D (2009) Effects of maternal panic disorder on mother-child interactions and relation to child anxiety and child self-efficacy. Arch Womens Ment Health 12:251–259CrossRef
78.
go back to reference Camras LA, Ribordy S, Hill J, Martino S, Sachs V, Spaccarelli S et al (1990) Maternal facial behavior and the recognition and production of emotional expression by maltreated and non-maltreated children. Dev Psychol 26:304–312CrossRef Camras LA, Ribordy S, Hill J, Martino S, Sachs V, Spaccarelli S et al (1990) Maternal facial behavior and the recognition and production of emotional expression by maltreated and non-maltreated children. Dev Psychol 26:304–312CrossRef
79.
go back to reference Denham SA, Zoller D, Couchoud EA (1994) Socialization of preschooler’s emotional understanding. Dev Psychol 30:928–936CrossRef Denham SA, Zoller D, Couchoud EA (1994) Socialization of preschooler’s emotional understanding. Dev Psychol 30:928–936CrossRef
80.
go back to reference Burnstein M, Ginsberg GS (2010) The effect of parental modeling of anxious behaviors and cognitions in school-aged children: an experimental pilot study. Behav Res Ther 48:506–515CrossRef Burnstein M, Ginsberg GS (2010) The effect of parental modeling of anxious behaviors and cognitions in school-aged children: an experimental pilot study. Behav Res Ther 48:506–515CrossRef
81.
go back to reference Bögels SM, van Dogen L, Muris PM (2003) Family influence on dysfunctional thinking in anxious children. Infant Child Dev 12:243–252CrossRef Bögels SM, van Dogen L, Muris PM (2003) Family influence on dysfunctional thinking in anxious children. Infant Child Dev 12:243–252CrossRef
82.
go back to reference Dunsmore JC, Her P, Halberstadt AG, Perez-Rivera M (2009) Parents’ beliefs about emotions and children’s recognition of parents’ emotions. J Nonverbal Behav 33:121–140PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Dunsmore JC, Her P, Halberstadt AG, Perez-Rivera M (2009) Parents’ beliefs about emotions and children’s recognition of parents’ emotions. J Nonverbal Behav 33:121–140PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
83.
go back to reference Wolfensberger SPA, Veltman DJ, Hoogendijk WJG, Boomsman DI, De Geus EJC (2008) Amygdala responses to emotional faces in twins discordant or concordant for the risk for anxiety and depression. NeuroImage 41:544–552CrossRefPubMed Wolfensberger SPA, Veltman DJ, Hoogendijk WJG, Boomsman DI, De Geus EJC (2008) Amygdala responses to emotional faces in twins discordant or concordant for the risk for anxiety and depression. NeuroImage 41:544–552CrossRefPubMed
84.
go back to reference Uhmann S, Beesdo-Baum K, Becker E, Hoyer J (2010) Specificity of interpersonal problems in generalized anxiety disorder versus other anxiety disorders and depression. J Nerv Ment Dis 198:846–851CrossRefPubMed Uhmann S, Beesdo-Baum K, Becker E, Hoyer J (2010) Specificity of interpersonal problems in generalized anxiety disorder versus other anxiety disorders and depression. J Nerv Ment Dis 198:846–851CrossRefPubMed
85.
go back to reference Bölte S, Feineis-Matthews S, Leber S, Kierks T, Hubl D, Poustka F (2002) The development and evaluation of a computer-based program to test and to teach the recognition of facial affect. Int J Circumpolar Health 61:61–68CrossRefPubMed Bölte S, Feineis-Matthews S, Leber S, Kierks T, Hubl D, Poustka F (2002) The development and evaluation of a computer-based program to test and to teach the recognition of facial affect. Int J Circumpolar Health 61:61–68CrossRefPubMed
86.
go back to reference Luby J, Lenze S, Tillman R (2012) A novel early intervention for preschool depression: findings from a pilot randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 53:313–322PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Luby J, Lenze S, Tillman R (2012) A novel early intervention for preschool depression: findings from a pilot randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 53:313–322PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
87.
go back to reference Wölwer W, Frommann N, Halfmann S, Piaszek A, Streit M, Gaebel W (2005) Remediation of impairments in facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: efficacy and specificity of a new training program. Schizophr Res 80:295–303CrossRefPubMed Wölwer W, Frommann N, Halfmann S, Piaszek A, Streit M, Gaebel W (2005) Remediation of impairments in facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: efficacy and specificity of a new training program. Schizophr Res 80:295–303CrossRefPubMed
88.
go back to reference Penton-Voak IS, Bate H, Lewis G, Munafo M (2012) Effects of emotion perception training on mood in undergraduate students: randomized controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry 200:1–3CrossRef Penton-Voak IS, Bate H, Lewis G, Munafo M (2012) Effects of emotion perception training on mood in undergraduate students: randomized controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry 200:1–3CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Impaired Facial Affect Perception in Unaffected Children at Familial Risk for Panic Disorder
Auteurs
Cynthia Bilodeau
Jacques Bradwejn
Diana Koszycki
Publicatiedatum
15-10-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Child Psychiatry & Human Development / Uitgave 5/2015
Print ISSN: 0009-398X
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3327
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-014-0513-z