Skip to main content

Welkom bij Scalda & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Scalda heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen.Je kunt de producten hieronder links aanschaffen en rechts inloggen.

Registreer

Schaf de BSL Academy aan: 

BSL Academy mbo AG

Eenmaal aangeschaft kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met Jan van der Velden.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

01-01-2015 | Original Article

Generality and specificity in cognitive control: conflict adaptation within and across selective-attention tasks but not across selective-attention and Simon tasks

Auteurs: Antonio L. Freitas, Sheri L. Clark

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

To explain how cognitive control is modulated contextually, Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, and Cohen (Psychol Rev 108:624–652, 2001) proposed that detecting information-processing conflict attenuates the disruptive influence of information-processing conflicts encountered subsequently, by which time appropriate cognitive-control mechanisms already will have been engaged. This conflict-adaptation hypothesis has motivated extensive programs of research while also attracting vigorous methodological critiques that highlight alternative accounts of trial n × trial n  1 sequential effects in cognitive-control tasks. Addressing those alternatives through precluding analyzing stimulus repetitions without creating any sort of confounds among any stimulus or trial characteristics, the present research observed significant conflict-adaptation effects within and across several selective-attention tasks. Moreover, across-task conflict-adaptation effects were largest when spanning tasks (i.e., a newly developed Stroop-trajectory task and a flanker task, which both require resolving conflict among stimulus elements) that presumably depend on the same mechanism of cognitive control (selective attention) than when spanning tasks that do not (i.e., the Stroop-trajectory task and a Simon task, the latter—but not former—of which requires resolving conflict between stimulus and response elements). These findings contribute to advancing beyond examining whether or not conflict adaptation exists to clarifying the conditions under which it is and is not observed.
Voetnoten
1
Another potentially important difference between the experiments of Freitas et al. (2007), and those of Mayr et al. (2003), is that the former experiments selected trials for presentation randomly without replacement at the level of blocks, whereas the latter experiments selected trials for presentation randomly with replacement (Mayr, 2008, personal communication). As reported herein, all experiments in this investigation selected trials for presentation randomly with replacement.
 
2
This 1,000 ms cutoff, rather than the 800 ms cutoff used in Experiment 1, was needed in this experiment and in Experiment 3 given the relatively slower responses in this experiment and in Experiment 3 (which is not surprising given that these two experiments each interspersed three different task manipulations across trials) and given our laboratory’s general practice of not removing from analysis more than around of 3 % of response times. To ensure that the substantive conclusions reported in this paper were not dependent on using different cutoff criteria across the three experiments, we re-analyzed the data from Experiment 1 using the 1,000 ms cutoff, which resulted in excluding 1.08 % of response times and yielded a trial n × trial n  1 congruency interaction on response time [F(1, 14) = 22.55, MSE = 97.41, p < 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.62] that did not differ appreciably from that reported in the main text (when using the 800 ms cutoff).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Braem, S., Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2011). Conflict adaptation by means of associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1662–1666. doi:10.1037/a0024385.PubMed Braem, S., Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2011). Conflict adaptation by means of associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1662–1666. doi:10.​1037/​a0024385.PubMed
go back to reference Carter, C. S., & Krug, M. K. (2012). Dynamic cognitive control and frontal—cingulate interactions. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (2nd ed., pp. 89–98). New York: Guilford Press. Carter, C. S., & Krug, M. K. (2012). Dynamic cognitive control and frontal—cingulate interactions. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (2nd ed., pp. 89–98). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. doi:10.3758/BF03203267.CrossRef Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. doi:10.​3758/​BF03203267.CrossRef
go back to reference Funes, M. J., Lupiáñez, J., & Humphreys, G. (2010). Analyzing the generality of conflict adaptation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 147–161. doi:10.1037/a0017598.PubMed Funes, M. J., Lupiáñez, J., & Humphreys, G. (2010). Analyzing the generality of conflict adaptation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 147–161. doi:10.​1037/​a0017598.PubMed
go back to reference Gordon, R. D., & Irwin, D. E. (2000). The role of physical and conceptual properties in preserving object continuity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 136–150. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.1.136.PubMed Gordon, R. D., & Irwin, D. E. (2000). The role of physical and conceptual properties in preserving object continuity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 136–150. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​26.​1.​136.PubMed
go back to reference Hazeltine, E., Lightman, E., Schwarb, H., & Schumacher, E. H. (2011). The boundaries of sequential modulations: evidence for set-level control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1898–1914. doi:10.1037/a0024662.PubMed Hazeltine, E., Lightman, E., Schwarb, H., & Schumacher, E. H. (2011). The boundaries of sequential modulations: evidence for set-level control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1898–1914. doi:10.​1037/​a0024662.PubMed
go back to reference Kan, I. P., Teubner-Rhodes, S., Drummey, A. B., Nutile, L., KrupaNovick, L., & Novick, J. M. (2014). To adapt or not to adapt: the question of domain-general control. Cognition (in press). Kan, I. P., Teubner-Rhodes, S., Drummey, A. B., Nutile, L., KrupaNovick, L., & Novick, J. M. (2014). To adapt or not to adapt: the question of domain-general control. Cognition (in press).
go back to reference Kleiman, T., Hassin, R. R., & Trope, Y. (2014). The control-freak mind: stereotypical biases are eliminated following conflict-activated cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General (in press). Kleiman, T., Hassin, R. R., & Trope, Y. (2014). The control-freak mind: stereotypical biases are eliminated following conflict-activated cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General (in press).
go back to reference Kopp, B., Mattler, U., Goertz, R., & Rist, F. (1996). N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99(1), 19–27. doi:10.1016/0921-884X(96)95617-9.PubMedCrossRef Kopp, B., Mattler, U., Goertz, R., & Rist, F. (1996). N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99(1), 19–27. doi:10.​1016/​0921-884X(96)95617-9.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kornblum, S., & Lee, J. W. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 855. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.21.4.855.PubMed Kornblum, S., & Lee, J. W. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 855. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​21.​4.​855.PubMed
go back to reference Kühn, S., Keizer, A. W., Colzato, L. S., Rombouts, S. B., & Hommel, B. (2011). The neural underpinnings of event-file management: evidence for stimulus-induced activation of and competition among stimulus-response bindings. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 896–904. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21485.PubMedCrossRef Kühn, S., Keizer, A. W., Colzato, L. S., Rombouts, S. B., & Hommel, B. (2011). The neural underpinnings of event-file management: evidence for stimulus-induced activation of and competition among stimulus-response bindings. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 896–904. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2010.​21485.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452. doi:10.1038/nn1051.PubMed Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 450–452. doi:10.​1038/​nn1051.PubMed
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 993–1000. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.993.PubMed Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 993–1000. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​18.​5.​993.PubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective, 65, 31–86. Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective, 65, 31–86.
go back to reference Soutschek, A., Taylor, P., Muller, H., & Schubert, T. (2013). Dissociable mechanisms resolve conflicts during perception and response selection: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 5647–5654.PubMedCrossRef Soutschek, A., Taylor, P., Muller, H., & Schubert, T. (2013). Dissociable mechanisms resolve conflicts during perception and response selection: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 5647–5654.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ullsperger, M., Bylsma, L. M., & Botvinick, M. M. (2005). The conflict adaptation effect: it’s not just priming. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(4), 467–472. doi:10.3758/CABN.5.4.467.CrossRef Ullsperger, M., Bylsma, L. M., & Botvinick, M. M. (2005). The conflict adaptation effect: it’s not just priming. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(4), 467–472. doi:10.​3758/​CABN.​5.​4.​467.CrossRef
go back to reference Wendt, M., Kluwe, R. H., & Peters, A. (2006). Sequential modulation of interference evoked by processing task-irrelevant stimulus features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 644–667. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.644.PubMed Wendt, M., Kluwe, R. H., & Peters, A. (2006). Sequential modulation of interference evoked by processing task-irrelevant stimulus features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 644–667. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​32.​3.​644.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Generality and specificity in cognitive control: conflict adaptation within and across selective-attention tasks but not across selective-attention and Simon tasks
Auteurs
Antonio L. Freitas
Sheri L. Clark
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0540-1