Within-Person Relationships
Contrary to the hypotheses guided by the Tripartite Influence Model (Thompson et al., 1999), at the level of within-person associations, increased appearance activity on social media, as compared to the usual level of appearance activity across waves, did not relate to such increases in social media-ideal internalization, appearance comparison, and body dissatisfaction in adolescents six months later. The only statistically significant results were that adolescents who more intensely internalized social media ideals subsequently reported elevated appearance comparison and appearance activity on social media. However, these associations occurred only from W2 to W3, limiting their interpretation as substantial and consistent. A sensitivity analysis further showed these associations as non-stable, especially the link from W2 social media-ideal internalization to W3 appearance activity. Importantly, the mediation associations from appearance activity on body dissatisfaction through the internalization of and comparison with social media ideals were not significant.
Overall, the present findings do not align with the expectations drawn from the Tripartite Influence Model (Thompson et al.,
1999), because the internalization and appearance comparison with social media ideals did not mediate the relationship between appearance activity on social media and heightened body dissatisfaction over time. More precisely, at a within-person level, appearance activity did not relate to increased body dissatisfaction at all. These results also diverge from the conclusions of cross-sectional studies that pointed to the media-ideal internalization and appearance comparison as mediators for the association between appearance activity on social media and body dissatisfaction in adolescents (Scully et al.,
2023; Wang et al.,
2019). Using three-wave longitudinal data from Australian adolescents aged 11–16, thin-ideal internalization and social media comparisons mediated the association of the use of visual social media platforms (i.e., Instagram, Snapchat) and engagement in photo-based activities on decreased body satisfaction among adolescents six months later, supporting the Tripartite Influence Model in the context of appearance-centered social media use (Jarman et al.,
2024). One of the reasons for the different results can be that Jarman et al. (
2024) employed the cross-lagged panel model (CLPM) without the random intercepts, which fails to distinguish between-person and within-person associations over time. Their results likely reflect stable differences between individuals rather than the within-person fluctuations captured in the present study. The CLPM has been criticized for conflating these two distinct types of effects (Hamaker et al.,
2015). Indeed, the current results revealed that appearance activity, internalization, appearance comparison, and body dissatisfaction are related differently at the between- and within-person levels, with associations found at the between-person level but not at the within-person level. Given these nuances within the social media effects, further research of this type is needed to elucidate the Tripartite Influence Model’s (Thompson et al.,
1999) processes in the relationship between appearance activity on social media and adolescents’ body dissatisfaction.
Nonetheless, these results are in line with other longitudinal research (employing the RI-CLPM as this study did), showing that within-person changes in interactions with appearance content may not consistently relate to later body dissatisfaction in adolescents (Schreurs & Vandenbosch,
2022). It has been suggested that viewing, liking, and commenting on appearance-related content have become so habitual for adolescents that they may already be desensitized to its effects (Schreurs & Vandenbosch,
2022). In addition, the impact on body dissatisfaction may become evident when key variables, such as self-objectification, media-ideal internalization, and appearance comparison, are taken into account (Schreurs & Vandenbosch,
2022). While the desensitization effect might explain these and prior null results, this study did not support the role of the key process variables, particularly the internalization and comparison with social media ideals because neither increased subsequent body dissatisfaction.
Another idea is that appearance activity on social media might only affect adolescents’ body dissatisfaction when it involves relevant friends and peers. The study did not distinguish between interactions with appearance content posted by close friends versus more distant users. Since peer influence on appearance-oriented attitudes is particularly significant during adolescence (Choukas-Bradley et al.,
2022), seeing friends promote idealized images may have a greater impact on body image than seeing similar content from unknown individuals. Relatedly, information about the gender (mis)match between the users posting the idealized images and the adolescents engaging with these images might provide valuable insight. Though evidence on this topic remains limited, social comparison theory implies that adolescents might be more influenced by the images that show similar targets, like sharing their gender (Gerber et al.,
2018). In follow-up research, it is important to differentiate who posted the content and what specifically adolescents engage with, given the increasingly diverse appearance-centered social media landscape. Asking adolescents about their engagement over the preceding few months, as the current study did, may not capture these details accurately. Experience sampling and data donation methods could be particularly useful for this purpose.
With that said, the null results should also be interpreted in light of the attrition rates. Attrition was relatively high in this study, with fewer than half of the initial sample of adolescents participating in the final wave. The attrition analysis further showed that the drop-out from the second to the third waves was influenced by the frequency of appearance activity and appearance comparison. Adolescents who were more engaged with appearance activity and less inclined toward appearance comparisons were more likely to drop out of the study. The results should be interpreted with caution, considering the potential bias introduced by the attrition and the specific characteristics of the remaining sample.
In addition, this study did not discover that body dissatisfaction, social media-ideal internalization, and appearance comparison would consistently drive engagement with appearance activity on social media. Social media-ideal internalization led to increased appearance comparisons and appearance activity on social media only from W2 to W3. However, it is important to remember the results of the attrition analysis, which showed the attrition rates to be non-random from the second to the third wave of data collection, limiting the generalizability. A sensitivity analysis further pointed to the instability of the association between W2 social media-ideal internalization and W3 appearance activity. Bearing in mind the inconsistency of these associations, such results potentially hint at the possible bidirectional interplay between appearance activity on social media and body image, as well as at a sequential, rather than solely parallel role, of the Tripartite Influence Model’s (Thompson et al.,
1999) mediating processes (Rodgers,
2016). While these findings align with some prior research (Jarman et al.,
2024; Rodgers et al.,
2015), due to their inconclusiveness, further investigation is necessary to understand how body dissatisfaction, internalization, and appearance comparison drive appearance activity on social media.
Within the sensitivity analysis, a few differences emerged between appearance activity directed towards “others”, such as witnessing other users on social media posting, liking, and commenting on idealized content, and activity focused on the “self”, such as adolescents’ own posting, liking, and commenting on similar content. When solely considering the others-focused activities, at the within-person level, increased social media-ideal internalization at W2 predicted increased body dissatisfaction in W3, while this relationship did not appear in the main model or when considering self-oriented activity. On the other hand, increased self-oriented activity in W1 was linked with heightened appearance comparison in W2. These results should be taken cautiously, given their purely exploratory character and the lack of consistency across waves. At the same time, they could be viewed within the context of the recently disputed passive-active dichotomy in social media research. While the idea of passive (other-oriented) use leading to negative outcomes and active (self-oriented) use leading to positive outcomes is widespread, recent evidence opposes this view, because the dichotomy is not clear-cut and the specific outcomes depend on contextual and individual circumstances (Valkenburg et al.,
2022). The current findings potentially align with this perspective, because, for both “active” and “passive” forms of appearance activity, negative yet distinct outcomes came forward, namely body dissatisfaction and appearance comparison. Notably, the consequences of appearance-focused social media engagement for body image extend beyond this dichotomy, varying based on the type of content (e.g., selfies, favorable comments) and the form of engagement (e.g., editing, posting) (Vandenbosch et al.,
2022). Once again, such a reflection is hindered by the aforementioned limitations.
One aspect to consider is the conceptualization of appearance comparison and the internalization of ideals from social media. The questionnaire did not specifically ask about internalizing thin and muscular ideals, aiming instead to capture a broader spectrum of ideals, thin, muscular, or fit, that adolescents find attractive and that might exacerbate body dissatisfaction. However, it is possible that the internalization and comparison with more realistic body ideals that adolescents find attractive, yet more attainable, was captured as well, like facial ideals (e.g., perfect skin, full lips), that likely do not influence body shape dissatisfaction. Additionally, this study did not assess the direction of the comparisons, whether they were upward, likely reinforcing body dissatisfaction, or downward, potentially alleviating body dissatisfaction. This could partially explain why the association between appearance activity, internalization, comparison, and body dissatisfaction was not observed over time. As literature on social comparison suggests (Knobloch-Westerwick,
2015), interactions with idealized bodies might even promote body satisfaction in cases where self-improvement motives are present, as individuals may view beauty ideals as motivational.
On the methodology side, one explanation of the null results may lie in measuring appearance activity, internalization, appearance comparison on social media, and body dissatisfaction within the time intervals not sufficiently sensitive to capture changes in these variables. This reflection has already appeared in past media effects literature (Maes & Vandenbosch,
2023; Schreurs et al.,
2024), proposing that the challenge of detecting media effects may be caused by the fact that these effects arise over shorter (or longer) timeframes than those selected in studies. Intensified tendencies for the internalization of social media ideals and appearance comparison, and the subsequent body dissatisfaction, might emerge as the more immediate consequences of appearance activity on social media than after a six-month period. In that case, longitudinal designs with shorter time frames (e.g., experience sampling studies) could be a fruitful direction in this research area. That said, long-term longitudinal studies that span several years also present a valuable avenue. The present research examined appearance-related social media use and body image over a one-year period, which captures a relatively brief period of adolescent development. To gain a more comprehensive understanding of this relationship, future research could benefit from combining experience sampling studies and focusing on short-term effects, with long-term studies that span the full adolescence period.
Relatedly, a reflection on the importance of sensitivity to calendar moments has recently emerged in digital media effects literature (Vandenbosch et al.,
2025). The present study collected data in June (W1), December (W2), and the following June (W3), with significant within-person effects occurring only between W2 and W3. Regarding the topic of body image, adolescents may be more susceptible to the effects of digital media during the summer months due to wearing typically more revealing clothing (Vandenbosch et al.,
2025). This aspect could have underlaid the observed inconsistent findings across waves and should be considered more thoroughly in follow-up research.
Differences Between Girls and Boys
Despite girls being more invested in appearance social media interactions (Paddock & Bell,
2021) and because they are to a higher extent socialized to accomplish attractive ideals (Choukas-Bradley et al.,
2022), the within-person associations between appearance activity, internalization, appearance comparison on social media, and body dissatisfaction did not differ between girls and boys. This is consistent with previous research, which showed that the connection between the time spent on appearance-centered social media and lower body satisfaction, as mediated by social and appearance comparison and thin-ideal internalization, was the same for adolescent girls and boys aged 11–17 (Jarman et al.,
2021b,
2024). Similarly, time spent on social media and appearance-focused activities related to negative body image across studies, regardless of gender (Saiphoo & Vahedi,
2019). It should be noted that, while the present study assessed binary categories with which the adolescents identified, variations in the terminology of “sex” and “gender” across studies may somewhat limit comparability. An increasing number of studies highlight that adolescents with diverse gender identities have unique body image experiences (McGuire et al.,
2016). These distinct experiences may also shape how they engage with appearance activity on social media and the subsequent impact on body dissatisfaction. For instance, gender-diverse adolescents might interact with different appearance ideals, as androgynous body ideal, combining both masculine and feminine traits, is particularly prominent among non-binary and transgender people (Galupo et al.,
2021). Future research is encouraged to look beyond the gender binary and include adolescents with diverse gender and sexual identities, as these groups are often overlooked, as well as into the person-specific effects of individual adolescents (Valkenburg et al.,
2024). The need to account for individual moderators, such as different motivations for internet use (e.g., seeking appearance feedback, social support) has already been emphasized (Rodgers,
2016). Within the context of these findings, and in line with the uses and gratification perspective, the appearance activity possibly reinforces body dissatisfaction for some adolescents while having no impact, or even promoting body satisfaction for others, that is dependent on individual experiences and motivations.
Limitations and Future Directions
This study followed the Tripartite Influence Model of body image (Thompson et al.,
1999), employing a longitudinal study design and a robust sample of adolescents. However, further research may consider looking at multiple aspects of adolescent body image and other essential processes that link it to appearance-related social media use. Adolescent body image encompasses not only body (dis)satisfaction but also positive body image characteristics, such as body appreciation and resilience to appearance ideals and negative appearance feedback (Maes et al.,
2021). With that said, positive body image can serve as a crucial protective factor for well-being, extending beyond body dissatisfaction (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow,
2015). To fully understand the impact of appearance-related activities on social media on adolescent body image, it is essential to include positive body image characteristics in future research. Regarding sampling bias, attrition from W2 to W3 was related to the reported frequency of appearance activity and appearance comparison. This limitation should be considered when interpreting the results.
Although age was included as a covariate, the present study did not delve into specific developmental factors that might influence the relationships between appearance activity and body dissatisfaction. In particular, pubertal status may play an important role, because early- or late-maturing adolescents can experience varying levels of body dissatisfaction. For example, early maturation appears to increase body dissatisfaction in girls but reduce it in boys, due to different body ideals (Ricciardelli & Yager,
2016). Consequently, pubertal timing could shape the responses to appearance-focused content on social media, because it motivates adolescents to different social media behaviors (Swirsky et al.,
2022). Exploring the different developmental trajectories of engagement with appearance activity and body dissatisfaction, as influenced by pubertal status, could provide valuable insights in future research.
Other limitations pertain to the context of data collection. First, the data were collected via online questionnaires distributed in an agency’s online panel. This approach provided limited control over the questionnaire completion process, because adolescents’ parents received the questionnaire link via email and adolescents could fill it out at anytime and anywhere. Consequently, some adolescents may have paid limited attention to the questionnaire or completed it under the influence of peers or their parents. Furthermore, parents acted as gatekeepers, who could have excluded their offspring from the study after reviewing the questionnaire. This may have led to the underrepresentation of certain groups of adolescents, such as those with past negative experiences with social media or body image concerns, limiting the generalizability of the findings. Second, the data were collected during the COVID-19 pandemic, introducing unique circumstances that affected adolescents’ social media use and other areas of their lives. In Czechia, the months leading up to the first wave of data collection in June 2021 were marked by a ban of offline schooling (Slabá,
2022). Overall, there were accounts of increased social media use among adolescents during the pandemic (Marciano et al.,
2022). At the same time, the measures implemented, such as physical distancing, posed significant challenges to youth’s mental health (Meherali et al.,
2021). While body dissatisfaction rates were relatively low in the current study sample, these findings should be considered in light of the pandemic circumstances, and further replication research with other samples is needed.
Another limitation relates to the way that body dissatisfaction was measured. The scale was not administered in its entirety, as items that assessed the same body parts in a reverse manner were omitted. The scale has also been validated for adolescents aged 13 and older (Garner,
2004). Given that the present study also included early adolescents as young as 11, it is important to acknowledge potential limitations in measuring body dissatisfaction within this age group. For instance, young adolescents who have not yet experienced pubertal changes in body shape and weight may not interpret the scale items in the same way, potentially limiting the accuracy of body dissatisfaction measurement. Furthermore, the measurement did not differentiate between specific aspects, such as thinness, body weight, body fat, or muscularity. Recently, concerns have been raised about the inadequate measurement of body image and disordered eating in adolescent boys with instruments that are not specifically designed to capture key aspects of boys’ body image (Hansson & Schmidt,
2025). For boys in particular, muscularity and body fat are significant components of their body image (Tylka,
2011). These aspects were not assessed and, as a result, some crucial components of adolescent boys’ body dissatisfaction may have gone undetected, potentially diminishing the connection with appearance activity. A more complex approach to adolescents’ body dissatisfaction with a particular focus on the adequate content validity of the utilized scales is essential in future research. Furthermore, a greater nuance in the conceptualization and measurement of appearance activity, appearance comparison, and internalization variables could be valuable for future research. For instance, distinguishing between upward and downward comparison processes, as well as the exploration of the internalization of diverse body types, such as thin versus fit ideals, might show different links between appearance activity and body dissatisfaction. Adolescents can also engage in appearance activity with different user targets, such as friends versus more distant users, and interact with different kinds of content that serves different purposes, like fitspiration to promote fitness and lean ideals versus body positivity to encourage body acceptance. Clarifying these nuances is therefore an important task for follow-up research.