Psychopathy is argued to be the first personality disorder to be identified in psychiatry (Millon et al.,
1998). Despite its popularity, the psychopathy construct has been a source of controversy at both conceptual and operational levels for more than a century. What constitutes psychopathy (e.g., Cooke et al.,
2007; Hicks & Drislane,
2018), which aspects of psychopathy are central to the construct (e.g., Arrigo & Shipley,
2001; Cooke & Sellbom,
2019), and whether psychopathy is a dimensional or categorical structure (see Harris et al.,
2001; Sellbom & Drislane,
2020, for further details) are some of the matters under debate in the literature. Historically, the major emphasis of psychopathy is on the behavioral deviant manifestations versus tendencies towards dysfunctional affective and interpersonal traits (Venables et al.,
2014). The evidence that has emerged in the last decade characterizes psychopathic personality as a constellation of affective deficits (e.g., callousness, lack of remorse, and empathy); interpersonal deficits (e.g., deceitfulness, grandiosity, manipulativeness); and corresponding behavioral manifestations (e.g., anti-social acts, aggression, quick temperedness; Skeem et al.,
2011). In the current study, we focused on the Comprehensive Assessment of Psychopathic Personality (CAPP; Cooke et al.,
2004,
2012), which is the most recent attempt to integrate the scientific and clinical literature of psychopathy into a comprehensive concept map.
Controversies aside, there is a general consensus that socio-cultural factors have a significant influence on shaping psychopathic personality and the integral part of psychopathy assessment is deviations from social and cultural norms (Cooke & Michie,
1999). However, the construct of psychopathic personality is mainly formulated based on Western cultures. The literature is fraught with Western-based measures which have been mainly developed in the Euro-American cultures, but skeptics have warned that these measures have limited applicability for the study of Non-Western cultures, especially developing societies (Naidoo et al.,
2020; Sinha,
1990). To fill this gap, researchers have begun to examine the validation and applicability of the existing psychopathy construct and measurements in Non-Western societies such as the East Asian cultural context (Sea,
2018; Shou et al.,
2016,
2017,
2020; Sohn & Lee,
2016; Wang et al.,
2018), however, there are a few studies that have examined the psychopathy construct in Middle Eastern countries (Atari & Chegeni,
2016; Chegeni & Atari,
2016; Latzman et al.,
2015; Shariat et al.,
2010; see our introduction to the relevant literature later). As such, to develop a more universal psychology surrounding psychopathy, the current study was conducted to begin to fill this gap in the literature.
Comprehensive Assessment of Psychopathic Personality
The Comprehensive Assessment of Psychopathic Personality (CAPP; Cooke et al.,
2004,
2012) was developed as a conceptual map of psychopathic personality based on a dynamic personality-trait approach, comprising a semi-structured interview-based Institutional Rating Scale (CAPP-IRS; measurement of personality pathology and psychosocial adjustment in the past six months) and the Staff Rating Scale (CAPP-SRS; measurement of functional deficits as rated by practitioners), which have been merged into the CAPP Symptom Rating Scale (see Cooke et al.,
2021, for further details). Founded on the lexical hypothesis, similar to prominent ‘normal’ personality models such as the Big 5 (e.g., Goldberg,
1990), the CAPP model encompasses 33 psychopathic personality symptoms; each symptom (e.g., unreliable) reflects disruption of various personality functions and is described by three adjectives or adjectival phrases (e.g., undependable, untrustworthy, irresponsible; see Cooke & Logan,
2018). These symptoms can be thematically organized into six functional domains of psychopathy personality (i.e., Attachment, Behavioral, Cognitive, Dominance, Emotional, and Self) which gives the CAPP a hierarchical structure (Cooke et al.,
2012; Florez et al.,
2018; Kreis & Cooke,
2011; Kreis et al.,
2012; Sandvik et al.,
2012; Sellbom et al.,
2015). The CAPP is also a gender-neutral model intended to be used in different settings and be able to detect potential psychopathy personality changes over time with more focus on personality traits and less focus on antisocial and criminal behaviors (Cooke et al.,
2012; Florez et al.,
2015; Kreis & Cooke,
2011; Pederson et al.,
2010).
The content validity of the CAPP model is promising across different languages, cultures, gender, expertise, and lay populations (e.g., Florez et al.,
2015; Hoff et al.,
2012; Hoff et al.,
2014; Kreis et al.,
2012; Kreis & Cooke,
2011). The psychometric and structural properties of the CAPP have been also well established. Some researchers found support for the clinical utility (Pederson et al.,
2010); construct validity (Sandvik et al.,
2012); and content validity (e.g., Florez et al.,
2018; Hoff et al.,
2012; Kavish et al.,
2020; Kreis et al.,
2012; Sellbom et al.,
2015) of the CAPP model. Others have evaluated interview-based measures of CAPP (i.e., CAPP-SRS) in a sample of prison inmates and have demonstrated the reliability and robust psychometric properties of the CAPP model (Cooke et al.,
2021; Florez et al.,
2018).
To complement the available suite of CAPP operationalizations, Sellbom et al. (
2019) developed a new operationalization of the CAPP model, the CAPP Self-Report (CAPP-SR; see
https://capp-network.no/capp-sr/ for the formal CAPP-SR manual, Sellbom & Cooke,
2020). The CAPP-SR inventory has 99 items; 3 items for each of 33 CAPP symptoms. Sellbom et al. (
2019) developed the CAPP-SR through various latent modelling and classical test theory procedures and validated the scale across two university and community samples from two different English-Speaking countries (i.e., United States and New Zealand; Sellbom et al.,
2019). Overall, they found that the CAPP-SR scale has good criterion-related validity with the CAPP-Lexical Rating Scale (CAPP-LRS; Cooke et al.,
2004); promising convergent validity with those of other psychopathy personality measures; and sufficient incremental validity over the CAPP-LRS symptoms in capturing variance in other psychopathy personality measures. Furthermore, to inform the hierarchical interpretation of the CAPP model, Sellbom et al. (
2021) examined the higher-order factor structure of the CAPP-SR symptom scales in a large community sample of adults, university students, and offenders derived from three different countries (i.e., the United States, New Zealand, and Lithuania). Sellbom et al. (
2021) found a robust three-factor structure in the community sample (Antagonism/Meanness, Disinhibition, and Fearless Grandiosity) which was also confirmed across two other samples (i.e., university and offenders) using Exploratory Factor Analysis with targeted rotation. They reported promising convergent and discriminant validity of the resulting latent factor scores. They argued that this three-factor structure can be considered as an alternative hierarchical interpretation of CAPP-SR.
To date, the CAPP-SR has been translated into Chinese (Shou et al.,
2020) and Lithuanian (Sellbom et al.,
2020). To enhance cross-cultural research on psychopathic personality pathology, in the current study, we aimed to translate and cross-validate the CAPP-SR in a sample from Iran.
Psychopathy in Iran
Reviewing the existing literature published in both English and Persian, relevant studies on psychopathic traits among the Iranian population is sparse. In one study, Shariat et al. (
2010) examined the factor structure of the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL: SV) in a sample of Iranian prisoners and compared this population with the original standardization sample (as described in the PCL: SV manual; Hart et al.,
1995) from Canada and the United States. They found no consistent pattern of differences between the two samples when tested for structural invariance. Using Item Response Theory (IRT) analysis, however, Shariat et al. found that compared to the standardization sample, Arrogant and Deceitful Interpersonal Style (i.e., Superficial, Deceitful, and Grandiose items) had less discrimination power, whereas, Deficient Emotional Experience (i.e., Lacks Remorse and Lacks Empathy, but not Doesn’t Accept Responsibility) had higher differentiating power among the Iranian sample. Shariat et al. argued that cultural expectations, a collectivist culture, the duality of social life in Iran, authoritarianism, and the political atmosphere of suppression in Iranian society might underlie the differences in their findings.
In another study, Latzman et al. (
2015) examined the construct validity of self-reported psychopathy using the Arabic version of the Psychopathy Personality Inventory-Revised (PPI-R; Lilienfeld et al.,
2005) among undergraduate students in Egypt and Saudi Arabia, and then compared this population with a sample from the United States. Although Latzman et al. (
2015) observed a similar profile of psychopathy among Arabic-speaking Middle Eastern and American samples, they reported that in the Saudi Arabian sample, the PPI-R Cold-Heartedness scale was less strongly associated with other psychopathy traits. Latzman et al. argued that the PPI-R Cold-Heartedness scale might function differently in Western and Middle Eastern societies. In a similar study, Neumann et al. (
2012) examined psychopathic traits using the English version of the Self-Report Psychopathy scale (SRP; Paulhus et al.,
2009) in a very large sample recruited from 58 nations, including the Middle East. Neumann et al. found that compared to sample from the United States, Middle Eastern participants scored higher on the interpersonal domain of psychopathy, and scored moderately higher across affective, lifestyle, and antisocial aspects of psychopathy.
Like any other disorder, we need to consider ethnic and cultural factors when it comes to the psychological construct of psychopathy. To the best of our knowledge, there is only one research study in Iran that has investigated psychopathic traits, and there are few studies that have been conducted in non-Persian Middle Eastern countries. Given that Middle Eastern countries are distinct by language and culture and given the differences in the manifestation of psychopathic traits across different ethnicities (Hare,
1991), there is a need for context-specific studies to explore the differential expression of psychopathy traits across different societies. Moreover, the CAPP is the most comprehensive model of psychopathy personality that not only integrates various perspectives but also targets measuring affective-interpersonal traits rather than behavioral traits. Using this measure may help to understand which traits are necessary to be included in the concept of psychopathy. Thus, the current study is another attempt to examine whether the CAPP-SR can operationalize psychopathy similarly in a very different and distinct culture from those previously examined.
Results and Discussion
We conducted paired-sample correlations and
t-tests to examine the convergence between the English and Persian items of CAPP-SR (see online supplementary Table
S1 for complete details). Among the 99 items, 90 items indicated no significant difference in the means between the two language versions. The correlations between the English and Persian item pairs ranged from 0.46 (
p < .05) to 1.00, with a median of 0.80, and the great majority of items (i.e., 96 items) were associated with large effect sizes (
r’s ≥ 0.50). With respect to the 9 less agreed-upon items, the correlations between the English and Persian item pairs ranged from 0.65 to 0.89 (
p < .01), with a median of 0.79, and small to moderate effect sizes (Cohen’s
d ranged from 0.22 to 0.50). Overall, the results of Study 1 demonstrated good convergence for the Persian CAPP-SR items with their original English counterparts.
Study 2: Validating the Persian CAPP-SR
The purpose of Study 2 was to evaluate the reliability and validity of the Persian CAPP-SR in a sample of Iranian university students. To do this, we recruited university students studying in Iran to better understand how cultural and societal differences may affect the external correlates of psychopathic personality in a non-Western, non-clinical population. It is important to reiterate that one of our goals in the current study was to examine the relation between psychopathy personality and culturally-specific measures in Iran. To date, much of the research literature has used Western-designed measures to investigate psychopathy in the Middle Eastern and non-Western contexts. To bridge this gap, in this study, we used three scales that were originally developed in Iran to measure psychopathic-related constructs of addiction potential and social deviance among Iranian students.
Participants
The scale validation sample consisted of 366 Iranian university students who were recruited from the University of Tehran and the University of Ahvaz. No formal data on ethnicity were collected, but participants were all Iranian nationals with Persians accounting for the great majority of the Iranian population. Participants were recruited through invitations and snowball sampling. Participants completed an online battery of 9 questionnaires including the Persian CAPP-SR. Using embedded failed-attention questions (e.g., “If you are reading this statement, please respond ‘False’”), 57 participants were excluded from the experiment due to suspected random or non-content-based responding. The final sample (N = 309) consisted of 219 women and 90 men; no participants identified as gender diverse. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 41 years (M age = 25.79, SD = 5.58). With respect to educational attainment, 6.1% of participants reported having obtained an associate degree, 53.7% Bachelor’s, 28.8% Master’s, and 11.3% PhD. The research was reviewed and approved by both the University of Otago’s Human Ethics Committee (Health) and the equivalent Ethics Committee at the University of Ahvaz.
Measures
Next, we recruited 43 practicing Iranian clinical psychologists who ranged in age from 27 to 53 (M age = 33.16; SD = 8.41; 65% females) and presented them with a description of psychopathy personality. We then presented participants with the 20 forms of social deviation obtained in the first experiment and asked them to use a 10-point rating scale (1 = Not relevant to 10 = Very relevant) to rate how relevant each form of social deviance was to psychopathy personality in the context of Iran. The Intra-class Correlation Coefficient (ICC; two-way random effects model with the absolute agreement) used to assess reliability across raters was 0.87.
Overall, the social deviance dimensions that received the highest ratings of relevance to psychopathy personality in Iran were unreliability, vandalism, aggression, and substance abuse. However, because these four dimensions were already part of existing Persian measures that we used in the current study (i.e., Students’ Deviant Behaviors and the Iranian Addiction Potential Scale), we used the four next most highly-rated dimensions (infidelity, privacy violation, vilification, and violence against women) in the Social Deviance Checklist. In the final version that we used in Study 2, participants were presented with 20 questions (e.g., “Have you ever been in a romantic relationship with more than one person at a same time?”) and were asked to specify whether they had acted on each behavior using six response options (1 = No, 2 = Yes, once, to 6 = more than five times). The internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) for the total scale was 0.88.
Results
Table 1
Correlations between Persian CAPP-SR with LSRP, Persian CAPP-LRS and ICU scores
Persian CAPP-SR | Mean (SD) | ω | Total | Egocentricity | Callous | Antisocial | | Total | Callousness | Uncaring | Unemotional |
Total | 2.11 (0.27) | 0.91 | 0.79 | 0.59 | 0.40 | 0.64 | 0.54 | 0.50 | 0.57 | 0.22 | 0.17 |
Attachment | 1.72 (0.39) | 0.73 | 0.56 | 0.30 |
0.50
| 0.35 | 0.50 | 0.65 |
0.60
|
0.44
|
0.30
|
A1. Detached | 1.88 (0.57) | 0.59 | 0.33 | 0.15 |
0.29
| 0.29 | 0.49 | 0.38 |
0.32
|
0.21
|
0.28
|
A2. Uncommitted | 1.58 (0.59) | 0.57 | 0.53 | 0.40 |
0.40
| 0.29 | 0.27 | 0.52 |
0.53
|
0.31
|
0.19
|
A3. Unemphatic | 1.69 (0.51) | 0.38 | 0.35 | 0.17 |
0.42
| 0.14 | 0.37 | 0.53 |
0.50
|
0.33
|
0.25
|
A4. Uncaring | 1.72 (0.50) | 0.45 | 0.38 | 0.15 |
0.35
| 0.30 | 0.22 | 0.46 |
0.38
|
0.43
|
0.13
|
Behavioral | 2.12 (0.33) | 0.73 | 0.64 | 0.41 | 0.30 |
0.63
| 0.47 | 0.40 | 0.46 | 0.21 | 0.07 |
B1. Lacks perseverance | 2.10 (0.57) | 0.47 | 0.40 | 0.15 | 0.16 |
0.51
| 0.37 | 0.27 | 0.23 | 0.25 | 0.07 |
B2. Unreliable | 2.35 (0.46) | 0.41 | 0.02 | − 0.03 | − 0.10 |
0.08
| 0.07 | 0.01 | 0.06 | − 0.01 | − 0.04 |
B3. Reckless | 2.20 (0.55) | 0.53 | 0.42 | 0.31 | 0.15 |
0.39
| 0.29 | 0.15 | 0.24 | 0.03 | − 0.03 |
B4. Restless | 2.30 (0.66) | 0.60 | 0.33 | 0.23 | 0.12 |
0.37
| 0.49 | 0.08 | 0.19 | − 0.05 | − 0.04 |
B5. Disruptive | 1.97 (0.58) | 0.43 | 0.48 | 0.29 | 0.31 |
0.45
| 0.36 | 0.44 | 0.40 | 0.36 | 0.15 |
B6. Aggressive | 1.80 (0.62) | 0.59 | 0.49 | 0.40 |
0.35
|
0.31
| 0.39 | 0.39 |
0.42
|
0.19
|
0.13
|
Cognitive | 2.11 (0.36) | 0.73 | 0.70 |
0.46
| 0.35 | 0.66 | 0.48 | 0.42 | 0.41 | 0.26 | 0.18 |
C1. Suspicious | 2.24 (0.53) | 0.55 | 0.48 |
0.38
| 0.24 | 0.37 | 0.33 | 0.23 | 0.21 | 0.09 | 0.17 |
C2. Lacks concentration | 2.27 (0.60) | 0.55 | 0.41 | 0.24 | 0.13 |
0.50
| 0.49 | 0.17 | 0.17 | 0.12 | 0.03 |
C3. Intolerant | 1.77 (0.59) | 0.57 | 0.58 |
0.45
|
0.36
| 0.43 | 0.14 | 0.34 |
0.39
| 0.16 | 0.09 |
C4. Inflexible | 2.12 (0.62) | 0.57 | 0.51 | 0.41 | 0.18 |
0.48
| 0.41 | 0.36 | 0.34 | 0.19 | 0.19 |
C5. Lacks planfulness | 2.14 (0.55) | 0.61 | 0.22 | − 0.05 | 0.18 |
0.29
| 0.43 | 0.25 | 0.17 | 0.30 | 0.08 |
Dominance | 2.10 (0.36) | 0.72 | 0.62 |
0.58
| 0.30 | 0.39 | 0.45 | 0.30 | 0.41 | 0.08 | 0.03 |
D1. Antagonistic | 1.95 (0.60) | 0.60 | 0.52 |
0.38
| 0.32 | 0.40 | 0.40 | 0.35 | 0.42 | 0.18 | 0.03 |
D2. Domineering | 2.48 (0.60) | 0.56 | 0.27 |
0.39
| − 0.02 | 0.15 | 0.38 | − 0.00 | 0.11 | − 0.08 | − 0.11 |
D3. Deceitful | 1.70 (0.56) | 0.48 | 0.40 |
0.22
| 0.39 | 0.20 | 0.28 | 0.44 | 0.37 | 0.34 | 0.21 |
D4. Manipulative | 2.04 (0.62) | 0.56 | 0.39 |
0.40
| 0.17 | 0.23 | 0.27 | 0.22 | 0.24 | 0.09 | 0.11 |
D5. Insincere | 2.38 (0.59) | 0.37 | 0.23 |
0.32
| 0.03 | 0.10 | 0.22 | − 0.03 | 0.06 | − 0.16 | − 0.03 |
D6. Garrulous | 2.07 (0.64) | 0.54 | 0.39 |
0.35
| 0.21 | 0.28 | 0.23 | 0.10 | 0.24 | − 0.06 | − 0.05 |
Emotional | 2.10 (0.30) | 0.57 | 0.62 | 0.41 |
0.43
| 0.46 | 0.36 | 0.54 |
0.50
|
0.30
|
0.34
|
E1. Lacks anxiety | 2.64 (0.60) | 0.49 | 0.07 | 0.23 |
0.00
| − 0.13 | 0.30 | 0.03 |
0.16
|
− 0.11
|
− 0.07
|
E2. Lacks pleasure | 2.08 (0.62) | 0.65 | 0.14 | − 0.07 |
0.15
| 0.28 | 0.37 | 0.19 |
0.02
|
0.22
|
0.25
|
E3. Lacks emotional depth | 2.03 (0.60) | 0.53 | 0.42 | 0.29 |
0.24
| 0.34 | 0.35 | 0.45 |
0.32
|
0.22
|
0.49
|
E4. Lacks emotional stability | 2.19 (0.62) | 0.67 | 0.50 | 0.31 | 0.22 |
0.56
| 0.33 | 0.21 | 0.22 | 0.11 | 0.08 |
E5. Lacks remorse | 1.57 (0.58) | 0.51 | 0.41 | 0.27 |
0.47
| 0.10 | 0.31 | 0.49 |
0.53
|
0.31
| 0.09 |
Self | 2.35 (0.36) | 0.76 | 0.56 |
0.54
| 0.12 | 0.49 | 0.47 | 0.19 | 0.36 | − 0.06 | − 0.00 |
S1. Self-centered | 2.17 (0.58) | 0.48 | 0.50 |
0.44
| 0.21 | 0.36 | 0.35 | 0.29 | 0.34 | 0.13 | 0.08 |
S2. Self-aggrandizing | 2.55 (0.66) | 0.60 | 0.18 |
0.27
| − 0.03 | 0.10 | 0.30 | − 0.05 | 0.08 | − 0.20 | − 0.08 |
S3. Sense of uniqueness | 2.39 (0.62) | 0.59 | 0.36 |
0.39
| 0.11 | 0.24 | 0.42 | 0.12 | 0.24 | − 0.04 | − 0.03 |
S4. Sense of entitlement | 2.69 (0.61) | 0.55 | 0.30 |
0.40
| − 0.04 | 0.28 | 0.26 | − 0.04 | 0.12 |
− 0.28
| − 0.03 |
S5. Sense of invulnerability | 2.62 (0.54) | 0.50 | 0.13 |
0.31
| − 0.12 | 0.02 | 0.23 | − 0.08 | 0.08 | − 0.21 | − 0.13 |
S6. Self-justifying | 1.87 (0.60) | 0.60 | 0.48 | 0.30 | 0.26 |
0.47
| 0.23 | 0.34 | 0.42 | 0.17 | 0.03 |
S7. Unstable self-concept | 2.16 (0.61) | 0.50 | 0.39 | 0.15 | 0.10 |
0.58
| 0.40 | 0.25 | 0.22 | 0.16 | 0.13 |
Mean | | | 56.08 | 22.40 | 6.39 | 11.13 | 1.13 | 21.65 | 7.43 | 8.00 | 6.22 |
SD
| | |
7.59
|
3.89
|
2.40
|
2.50
|
0.89
|
8.85
|
5.56
|
3.62
|
2.84
|
With respect to LSRP, we found a large criterion correlation between CAPP-SR total score and LSRP total score (
r = .79). At the CAPP-SR domain level, all domains were largely correlated with theoretically corresponding LSRP factors (
r ≥ |0.50|), except for the emotional (
r = .43) and cognitive (
r = .46) domains. At the CAPP-SR symptom level, most of the correlations between CAPP-SR symptoms and LSRP factors were consistent with conceptual expectations, and their magnitude ranged from medium to large (See Table
1). There were, however, exceptions to this pattern. The correlations between CAPP-SR Detached (
r = .29), Lacks Emotional Depth (
r = .24), Lacks Pleasure (
r = .15), and Lacks Anxiety (
r = .00), with LSRP Callous did not reached our priori threshold for meaningfulness. Also, the correlations between CAPP-SR Lacks Planfulness (
r = .29) and Unreliable (
r = .08) with LSRP Antisocial failed to meet our threshold for meaningfulness. Moreover, the correlations between CAPP-SR Self-Aggrandizing (
r = .27) and Deceitful (
r = .22) with LSRP Egocentricity were other exceptions to our hypothesized pattern.
With respect to CAPP-LRS, we found a large criterion correlation between the CAPP-SR total score and CAPP-LRS total score (
r = .54). At the CAPP-SR domain level, all domains were correlated with their direct counterparts of CAPP-LRS, and their magnitudes ranged from medium to large. The largest correlations were between CAPP Attachment domains (
r = .50) and the lowest correlation was between CAPP Emotional domains (
r = .36). At the CAPP-SR symptom level, the criterion correlations were more variable. Twelve CAPP-SR symptoms did not reach our priori threshold for meaningfulness (
r ≥ |0.30|), with the majority being in the 0.22-0.29 range (See Table
1). The highest correlations were between Detached, Restless, and Lacks Concentration scales (
rs = 0.49), and the lowest correlations, that met the threshold for meaningfulness, were between Lacks Anxiety and Self-Aggrandizing scales (
rs = 0.30). Unexpectedly, the CAPP-SR Unreliable scale showed no criterion correlation with neither the LSRP total and its three factors nor with the corresponding CAPP-LRS.
Table 2
Correlations between Persian CAPP-SR and psychopathic-relevant personality measures
Persian CAPP-SR | Total | Active | Passive |
BIS
|
SDC
|
NPI
| Total | Substance abuse | Vandalism | Educational Cheating | Aggression | |
Total | 0.56 | 0.53 | 0.45 | 0.48 | 0.43 | 0.31 | 0.40 | 0.30 | 0.29 | 0.21 | 0.43 | |
Attachment | 0.40 | 0.40 | 0.21 | 0.34 | 0.32 | 0.14 | 0.29 | 0.23 | 0.29 | 0.08 | 0.31 | |
A1. Detached | 0.20 | 0.18 | 0.23 | 0.09 | 0.09 | 0.00 | 0.07 | 0.02 | 0.13 | − 0.02 | 0.12 | |
A2. Uncommitted | 0.41 | 0.42 | 0.17 | 0.33 |
0.35
| 0.24 | 0.30 | 0.24 | 0.26 |
0.14
| 0.28 | |
A3. Unemphatic | 0.27 | 0.28 | 0.09 | 0.21 | 0.27 | 0.07 | 0.27 | 0.25 | 0.22 | 0.07 | 0.27 | |
A4. Uncaring | 0.28 | 0.29 | 0.10 | 0.34 | 0.24 | 0.10 | 0.22 | 0.19 | 0.22 | 0.05 | 0.21 | |
Behavioral |
0.51
|
0.49
| 0.39 |
0.56
|
0.39
| 0.17 |
0.36
|
0.26
|
0.26
| 0.20 |
0.39
| |
B1. Lacks perseverance |
0.35
|
0.32
| 0.38 |
0.46
|
0.21
| 0.03 |
0.16
| 0.10 |
0.09
| 0.13 |
0.17
| |
B2. Unreliable |
− 0.04
|
− 0.06
| 0.02 | 0.11 | − 0.05 | − 0.03 | − 0.03 | − 0.00 | − 0.03 |
− 0.05
| − 0.02 | |
B3. Reckless |
0.28
|
0.27
| 0.24 |
0.41
|
0.27
| 0.15 |
0.25
|
0.20
|
0.19
| 0.17 |
0.22
| |
B4. Restless |
0.30
|
0.28
| 0.26 |
0.30
|
0.23
| 0.10 |
0.16
|
0.11
|
0.14
| 0.08 |
0.17
| |
B5. Disruptive |
0.36
|
0.35
| 0.22 |
0.37
|
0.31
| 0.11 |
0.35
| 0.26 |
0.28
| 0.18 |
0.37
| |
B6. Aggressive |
0.42
|
0.44
| 0.17 | 0.25 |
0.30
| 0.17 |
0.30
| 0.20 |
0.19
| 0.14 |
0.39
| |
Cognitive |
0.49
| 0.45 |
0.49
|
0.50
| 0.38 | 0.22 | 0.34 |
0.25
| 0.25 | 0.19 | 0.35 | |
C1. Suspicious | 0.23 | 0.19 | 0.30 | 0.21 | 0.24 | 0.23 | 0.15 | 0.11 | 0.09 | 0.07 | 0.17 | |
C2. Lacks concentration |
0.30
| 0.25 |
0.44
|
0.40
| 0.22 | 0.07 | 0.17 | 0.11 | 0.12 |
0.15
|
0.17
| |
C3. Intolerant | 0.43 | 0.40 | 0.36 | 0.29 |
0.35
| 0.26 | 0.28 | 0.26 | 0.22 | 0.08 | 0.30 | |
C4. Inflexible | 0.31 | 0.30 | 0.22 | 0.24 | 0.25 | 0.13 |
0.24
| 0.17 | 0.18 | 0.15 |
0.23
| |
C5. Lacks planfulness |
0.27
|
0.26
| 0.21 |
0.44
|
0.15
| − 0.01 |
0.22
|
0.15
| 0.17 |
0.15
|
0.21
| |
Dominance | 0.40 | 0.38 | 0.28 | 0.29 |
0.31
|
0.32
| 0.30 | 0.21 | 0.22 | 0.19 | 0.31 | |
D1. Antagonistic | 0.31 | 0.31 | 0.20 | 0.28 | 0.21 | 0.09 | 0.23 | 0.15 | 0.19 | 0.09 | 0.28 | |
D2. Domineering | 0.13 | 0.13 | 0.09 | 0.03 | 0.10 |
0.28
| 0.11 | 0.11 | 0.04 | 0.11 | 0.07 | |
D3. Deceitful | 0.29 | 0.30 | 0.13 | 0.22 |
0.35
| 0.12 | 0.32 | 0.22 | 0.32 |
0.15
| 0.32 | |
D4. Manipulative | 0.26 | 0.26 | 0.13 | 0.16 | 0.17 | 0.15 | 0.17 | 0.10 | 0.15 | 0.05 | 0.22 | |
D5. Insincere | 0.12 | 0.10 | 0.15 | 0.09 | 0.15 | 0.25 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.06 | 0.12 | 0.05 | |
D6. Garrulous | 0.28 | 0.25 | 0.30 | 0.24 | 0.12 |
0.23
| 0.16 | 0.12 | 0.02 | 0.15 | 0.16 | |
Emotional | 0.42 | 0.40 | 0.33 | 0.29 | 0.33 | 0.15 | 0.35 | 0.31 | 0.22 | 0.17 | 0.35 | |
E1. Lacks anxiety | 0.07 | 0.11 | − 0.12 | − 0.01 | 0.06 | 0.19 | 0.12 | 0.11 | 0.05 | 0.12 | 0.10 | |
E2. Lacks pleasure |
0.14
| 0.09 |
0.30
| 0.08 | 0.00 | − 0.10 | 0.03 | 0.09 | 0.02 | − 0.05 | 0.01 | |
E3. Lacks emotional depth | 0.31 | 0.28 | 0.28 | 0.11 | 0.19 | − 0.03 | 0.20 | 0.15 | 0.14 | 0.13 | 0.19 | |
E4. Lacks emotional stability |
0.28
| 0.24 |
0.34
|
0.30
|
0.24
| 0.07 | 0.21 | 0.12 | 0.07 | 0.17 |
0.27
| |
E5. Lacks remorse | 0.25 | 0.27 | 0.00 | 0.25 |
0.33
| 0.26 |
0.31
| 0.30 | 0.28 | 0.06 |
0.32
| |
Self | 0.42 | 0.39 | 0.36 | 0.29 | 0.30 |
0.35
| 0.26 | 0.19 | 0.15 | 0.14 | 0.30 | |
S1. Self-centered | 0.36 | 0.35 | 0.27 | 0.26 | 0.31 |
0.26
| 0.24 | 0.24 | 0.19 | 0.07 | 0.22 | |
S2. Self-aggrandizing | 0.18 | 0.18 | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.15 |
0.37
| 0.10 | 0.06 | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.13 | |
S3. Sense of uniqueness | 0.24 | 0.23 | 0.13 | 0.07 | 0.15 |
0.34
| 0.18 | 0.15 | 0.11 | 0.09 | 0.18 | |
S4. Sense of entitlement | 0.16 | 0.13 | 0.22 | 0.09 | 0.11 |
0.29
| 0.06 | − 0.06 | 0.00 | 0.15 | 0.15 | |
S5. Sense of invulnerability | 0.08 | 0.09 | − 0.03 | − 0.02 | 0.07 |
0.17
| 0.14 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.13 | 0.16 | |
S6. Self-justifying |
0.41
|
0.37
| 0.40 |
0.38
|
0.27
|
0.02
|
0.24
|
0.22
|
0.18
| 0.08 |
0.25
| |
S7. Unstable self-concept | 0.34 | 0.29 | 0.44 | 0.34 | 0.17 | 0.01 | 0.13 | 0.11 | 0.07 | 0.04 | 0.16 | |
Mean | 29.18 | 17.73 | 11.08 | 61.93 | 0.54 | 4.47 | 1.59 | 1.33 | 1.30 | 2.61 | 1.77 | |
SD
|
19.82
|
17.40
|
4.95
|
9.99
|
0.69
|
3.50
|
0.66
|
0.65
|
0.73
|
1.40
|
1.05
| |
As shown in the tables, for the most part, the CAPP-SR domains and symptoms converged with their conceptually hypothesized scales on the ICU, IAPS, BIS, and SDC. There were, however, some exceptions to this pattern. For example, CAPP-SR Lacks Anxiety, Lacks Pleasure, and Sense of Entitlement did not correlate to a meaningful degree with their conceptually-expected subscales on the ICU. Although the CAPP-SR Attachment domain was significantly correlated, at a medium effect size, with ICU Unemotional, the correlations between the Attachment symptoms and ICU Unemotional did not reach our threshold for meaningfulness. With respect to correlations with Active and Passive Addiction Potential, CAPP-SR Reckless, Restless, and Lacks Planfulness scores did not meet our significance threshold for meaningfulness with their respective conceptually-related scales. With respect to NPI, CAPP-SR Self-Aggrandizing and Sense of Uniqueness were the only symptom scales that reached our threshold for meaningfulness with their conceptually-related scales. With respect to Social Deviance Behaviors (i.e., SDC and SDB measures), CAPP-SR Uncommitted, Disruptive, Aggressive, Intolerant, Deceitful, and Lacks Remorse reached our threshold for meaningfulness with their conceptually-related scales. However, CAPP-SR Lacks Perseverance, Reckless, Restless, Lacks Planfulness, Lacks Emotional Stability, and Self-Justifying did not reach our threshold for meaningfulness with their conceptually-related scales. Again, CAPP-SR Unreliable scale did not show any correlation with any of the psychopathic-related personality scales.
We also examined the amount of variance of criterion measure (i.e., LSRP), and other external psychopathy personality measures (i.e., ICU, IAPS, BIS, SDC, NPI, and SDB) that could be accounted for by CAPP-SR symptom scales, and also, which of the latter scales contributed uniquely to these predictions. For this purpose, we conducted a series of regression analyses with the strategy of entrance condition that only those hypothesized CAPP-SR symptoms that reached our threshold for meaningfulness (
r ≥ |0.30|) in the previous bivariate analyses were included in the models. The results of the regression analyses are reported in Table
3.
Table 3
Multiple regression analyses predicting external psychopathy personality measures
A1. Detached | -- | -- | -- | | 0.04 | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
A2. Uncommitted | -- | 0.11 | -- | |
0.21
| 0.09 | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | 0.10 | -- | | -- |
A3. Unemphatic | -- |
0.16
| -- | |
0.19
| 0.10 | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
A4. Uncaring | -- | 0.10 | -- | | 0.05 |
0.31
| -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
B1. Lacks perseverance | -- | -- |
0.12
| | -- | -- | -- | |
0.17
| -- | |
0.21
| -- | -- | | -- |
B2. Unreliable | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
B3. Reckless | -- | -- | 0.07 | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | |
0.22
| -- | -- | | -- |
B4. Restless | -- | -- |
0.08
| | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | |
0.11
| -- | -- | | -- |
B5. Disruptive | -- | -- | 0.02 | | -- | -- | -- | | 0.08 | -- | |
0.11
| 0.10 | -- | |
0.22
|
B6. Aggressive | -- | 0.06 | − 0.02 | | 0.09 | -- | -- | |
0.33
| -- | | -- | 0.05 | -- | |
0.22
|
C1. Suspicious |
0.13
| -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
C2. Lacks concentration | -- | -- |
0.15
| | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
0.34
| | 0.09 | -- | -- | | -- |
C3. Intolerant |
0.15
| 0.12 | -- | | 0.08 | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- |
0.13
| -- | | -- |
C4. Inflexible | -- | -- |
0.19
| | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
C5. Lacks planfulness | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | |
0.26
| -- | -- | | -- |
D1. Antagonistic |
0.10
| -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
D2. Domineering |
0.14
| -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
D3. Deceitful | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- |
0.15
| -- | | -- |
D4. Manipulative |
0.15
| -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
D5. Insincere | 0.03 | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
D6. Garrulous |
0.11
| -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
E1. Lacks anxiety | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
E2. Lacks pleasure | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
0.21
| | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
E3. Lacks emotional depth | -- | -- | -- | | 0.06 | -- |
0.49
| | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
E4. Lacks emotional stability | -- | -- |
0.20
| | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
0.19
| | − 0.03 | -- | -- | | -- |
E5. Lacks remorse | -- |
0.22
| -- | |
0.21
| 0.10 | -- | | -- | -- | | -- |
0.12
| -- | |
0.16
|
S1. Self-centered | 0.07 | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
S2. Self-aggrandizing | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- |
0.25
| | -- |
S3. Sense of uniqueness | − 0.05 | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- |
0.20
| | -- |
S4. Sense of entitlement |
0.13
| -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
S5. Sense of invulnerability |
0.15
| -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
S6. Self-justifying | -- | -- | 0.06 | | -- | -- | -- | |
0.16
| -- | | 0.05 | -- | -- | | -- |
S7. Unstable self-concept | -- | -- |
0.29
| | -- | -- | -- | | -- | -- | | -- | -- | -- | | -- |
F
|
22.21
|
24.02
|
47.68
| |
31.62
|
23.14
|
100.62
| |
32.20
|
40.51
| |
29.29
|
14.78
|
30.96
| |
29.52
|
R
2
|
0.45
|
0.32
|
0.61
| |
0.45
|
0.23
|
0.24
| |
0.29
|
0.28
| |
0.43
|
0.22
|
0.16
| |
0.22
|
As shown in Table
3, the hypothesized symptoms explained the substantial amount of variance of the LSRP subscales scores (
R2 = 0.32-0.61). The CAPP-SR symptoms Suspicious, Intolerant, Antagonistic, Domineering, Manipulative, Garrulous, Sense of Entitlement, and Sense of Invulnerability remained significant predictors for LSRP Egocentricity. LSRP Callous was uniquely predicted by CAPP-SR Unempathic and Lacks Remorse. The significant predictors of LSRP Antisocial were CAPP-SR Lacks Perseverance, Restless, Lacks Concentration, Inflexible, Lacks Emotional Stability, and Unstable Self-Concept.
The amount of ICU score variance explained by CAPP-SR symptoms ranged from 0.23 to 0.45. The CAPP-SR symptoms significantly predicting ICU subscales were CAPP-SR Uncommitted, Unempathic, Uncaring, Lacks Emotional Depth, and Lacks Remorse. The amount of IAPS variance accounted for by CAPP-SR symptoms was 0.29 for Active addiction potential and 0.28 for Passive addiction potential. Active addiction potential was uniquely predicted by CAPP-SR Lacks Perseverance, Aggressive, and Self-justifying, and Passive addiction potential was uniquely predicted by CAPP-SR Lacks Concentration, Lacks Pleasure, and Lacks Emotional Stability. The amount of BIS variance explained by CAPP-SR symptoms was 0.43. BIS was uniquely predicted by CAPP-SR Lacks Perseverance, Reckless, Restless, Disruptive, and Lacks Planfulness. The amount of SDC variance explained by CAPP-SR symptoms was 0.22. The significant unique predictors of SDC were CAPP-SR Intolerant, Deceitful, and Lacks Remorse. The amount of NPI variance explained by CAPP-SR symptoms was 0.16, and CAPP-SR Self-Aggrandizing and Sense of Uniqueness were the unique significant predictors of NPI. The amount of SDB Aggression variance explained by CAPP-SR symptoms was 0.22, and CAPP-SR Disruptive, Aggressive, and Lacks Remorse were its significant predictors.
As can be seen in Table
3, the CAPP-SR symptoms remained significant in at least one regression model, with five exceptions; Detached, Unreliable, Insincere, Lacks Anxiety, and Self-centered. The results of both sets of regression analyses indicate that the Persian CAPP-SR appear to capture a substantial range of psychopathic personality traits.
Incremental validity. To examine the incremental validity of the Persian CAPP-SR symptom scores over the CAPP-LRS symptom scores in capturing variance in other external psychopathy personality measures’ total scores and subscales, we conducted two series of hierarchical regression analyses. In the first series of regressions, we examined the amount of additional variance accounted for by the CAPP-SR scales. To do so, we regressed the 33 CAPP-LRS symptoms in one block, followed by the 33 CAPP-SR symptoms in the subsequent block, to predict each criterion variable. To fully test the partitioning of variance in this model, we conducted a second series of regressions to examine the amount of incremental variance accounted for by CAPP-LRS scales, above and beyond CAPP-SR scale scores. To do so, we reversed the order of entry, with the 33 CAPP-SR symptoms in the first block and the 33 CAPP-LRS in the following block. These results are reported in Table
4.
Table 4
Hierarchical Regression Analyses Predicting Psychopathy Scores with the CAPP-SR and CAPP-LRS
LSRP | 0.28 | 2.34** | 0.44 | 7.87** | | 0.66 | 11.85** | 0.05 | 1.03 |
Egocentricity | 0.23 | 1.83* | 0.39 | 5.30** | | 0.55 | 7.28** | 0.08 | 1.08 |
Callous | 0.24 | 1.89* | 0.30 | 3.23** | | 0.47 | 5.30** | 0.07 | 0.75 |
Antisocial | 0.32 | 2.80** | 0.38 | 6.47** | | 0.64 | 10.56** | 0.06 | 1.11 |
ICU | 0.41 | 4.17** | 0.27 | 4.42** | | 0.59 | 8.82** | 0.09 | 1.48† |
Callousness | 0.39 | 3.79** | 0.27 | 3.90** | | 0.53 | 6.67** | 0.13 | 1.88* |
Uncaring | 0.27 | 2.19** | 0.34 | 4.37** | | 0.52 | 6.43** | 0.09 | 1.18 |
Unemotional | 0.36 | 3.32** | 0.25 | 3.17** | | 0.47 | 5.37** | 0.13 | 1.70* |
As shown in Table
4, the CAPP-SR outperformed the CAPP-LRS in all the predictions (mdn.
R2 = 0.54; range: 0.47-0.66 [CAPP-SR] vs. mdn.
R2 = 0.30; range: 0.23-0.41 [CAPP-LRS]). In terms of incremental validity, the CAPP-SR scales added a substantial amount of variance (mdn.
∆R2 = 0.32; range: 0.24-0.44) to the CAPP-LRS scales in all the instances. However, the CAPP-LRS accounted for far less variance above and beyond CAPP-SR scale scores (mdn.
∆R2 = 0.08; range: 0.05-0.13), with most increments being statistically non-significant.
General Discussion
The current study was an attempt to further globalize the conceptualization and operationalization of psychopathic personality disorder by translating and validating the CAPP-SR in an Iranian population—a nation and culture in which psychopathy research is grossly underrepresented. In Study 1, we translated the CAPP-SR into Persian, and the results of bilingual validation showed that the Persian version was equivalent in meaning to the English original. In Study 2, we examined the reliability and construct validity of the Persian CAPP-SR using a sample of Iranian university students. Overall, the current findings support the use of the Persian CAPP-SR as a reliable instrument with promising validity for measuring psychopathic personality and furthering research on the CAPP model among Persian speakers in Iran. These results were generally consistent with those findings of previous CAPP-SR validation studies (Sellbom et al.,
2019,
2020; Shou et al.,
2020).
More specifically, means and standard deviations of the Persian CAPP-SR symptoms scores were comparable to previous validation studies (e.g., Sellbom et al.,
2020; Shou et al.,
2020), although mean scores for two of the CAPP symptoms (i.e., Unreliable and Sense of Entitlement) tended to be somewhat elevated among the Iranian sample. This might reflect potential cultural differences among Iranian participants and those of English-speaking participants in previous studies. We return later to a discussion of potential cultural factors that might have caused these elevations.
The results of the reliability study were generally supportive of the internal consistency associated with the Persian CAPP-SR domain scale scores. The total scores of the Persian CAPP-SR exhibited high reliability (α = 0.91) which was similar to previous studies using an English-speaking university sample (
α = 0.92; Sellbom et al.,
2020). The reliability of the six CAPP-SR domains was relatively similar to the English-speaking university samples (Sellbom et al.,
2020; Sellbom & Cooke,
2020). Notably, the Emotional domain was the least internally coherent (α = 0.53), but this is the case in all the previous CAPP-SR (e.g., α = 0.66, Sellbom & Cooke,
2020) and CAPP-LRS studies (Kavish et al.,
2020).
Furthermore, in line with previous studies examining CAPP-SR in a non-Western context (Shou et al.,
2020), we found that not all of the symptoms of CAPP-SR had high internal consistency in the current sample. It is important to note that internal consistency is not only the function of scale characteristics but also it is associated with the characteristics of the population that is administered the scale (McCrae et al.,
2011). In this study, the Insincere symptom scale had low reliability among the Iranian sample, but this might not exclusively represent specific characteristics of the symptom scale. Alternatively, we argue that both our non-clinical sample that might relatively represent low levels of psychopathic traits and cultural factors that cause varying understanding of the scale statements might have impacted the values of internal consistency.
The results of the validity study showed that the CAPP-SR scales generally evidenced good criterion-related validity, with few exceptions to our hypothesized pattern as discussed earlier. Also, the CAPP-SR scales converged with other psychopathic-relevant personality measures in a manner that was generally in line with our conceptual expectations. Furthermore, the CAPP-SR outperformed the CAPP-LRS with respect to adding a variance in other psychopathic-relevant personality measures.
As we mentioned earlier, there were some unexpected findings where the CAPP-SR symptom scales did not correlate with the hypothesized measures. For example, in the Emotional Domain, Lacks Anxiety and Lacks Pleasure did not correlate with hypothesized LSRP Callous and ICU subscales (i.e., Callousness, Uncaring, Unemotional). Similarly, Lacks Emotional Depth did not correlate with LSRP Callous and ICU Uncaring. These findings are partially in line with previous CAPP-SR studies where Sellbom and colleagues (
2019) did not find a correlation between Lacks Anxiety and affective deficiency measures. Our findings are also partially in line with those studies that reported an inconsistent pattern of association between Callousness factors and hypothesized psychopathy-related constructs (Christian & Sellbom,
2016; Garofalo et al.,
2019; Salekin et al.,
2014; Sellbom,
2011). Moreover, the focus of LSRP Callousness is mostly other-oriented (e.g., lack of consideration for others and moral principles), whereas the items of Lacks Anxiety and Lacks Pleasure are self-oriented (e.g., lack of experiencing fear, stress, and enthusiasm). The lack of expected convergence between these measures might also be because Lacks Anxiety and Pleasure are less a result of affective and emotional deficiencies among Iranians. Another example is the Aggressive scale which did not correlate with ICU Uncaring and Unemotional. Taking Iranian culture into account, not only individuals can be high on Aggressive traits without necessarily being Uncaring and Unemotional. But also, aggressive behaviors are mostly explained and justified by the level of care and emotions that individuals hold about the subject of aggression. Future studies are needed in the context of Iran to elucidate whether these variables represent a cultural issue or are associated with experiencing ongoing societal and economic crises, and how this impacts the psychological profile of psychopathy personality among Iranians.
Of particular interest for this project, not all of the conceptually-relevant psychopathy traits predicted the Iranian culturally-specific measures (i.e., social deviance and addiction potential). For instance, grounded on the Western conceptualization of psychopathy, we hypothesized that all the six symptom scales of the CAPP-SR Behavioral dimension would be associated with active addiction potential and social deviance. Our hypothesis was partially supported because the Disruptive and Aggressive scales were associated with active addiction potential and the measures of social deviance. Lacks Perseverance was also associated with active addiction potential, but not with social deviance. However, Unreliable, Reckless, and Restless did not correlate with addiction potential or social deviance measures.
One possible explanation for these unexpected findings might involve the content of the scales. The items in the Addiction Potential Scale heavily focus on individuals’ attitudes about drugs, alcohol, and gambling, their tendency towards using drugs, and the associated sensation-seeking traits, whereas the CAPP-SR Reckless scale focuses on the rash, impetuous, risk-taking, and CAPP-SR Restless scale focuses on being overactive, energetic, and fidget (Sellbom & Cooke,
2020). Another possible explanation might involve our sample gender distribution. More than 70% of the current sample consisted of female students who represent significantly fewer psychopathy traits (Females
M = 2.08,
SD = 0.26; Males
M = 2.20;
SD = 0.28;
t(307) = 3.45,
p < .001, Cohen’s
d = 0.44), addiction potential (Females
M = 23.93,
SD = 22.65; Males
M = 41.98;
SD = 15.82;
t(307) = 7.98,
p < .001, Cohen’s
d = 0.99) and social deviance (Females
M = 1.52,
SD = 0.68; Males
M = 1.76;
SD = 0.58;
t(307) = 3.02,
p < .01, Cohen’s
d = 0.37) compared with males in Iran. This could have introduced range restrictions and potentially attenuated our observed correlations.
Cross-Cultural Implications for Psychopathy Construct in Iran
As we mentioned earlier, mean scores for Unreliable and Sense of Entitlement symptom scales tended to be somewhat elevated among the Iranian sample. Looking more closely at the three items that form the Sense of Entitlement symptom scale, the mean score for two of the items (i.e., “I deserve special treatment” and “I might be perceived as demanding, but I am also deserving”) were in a normal range. However, scores on one item related to assertiveness with entitled motivation (i.e., “I often find that I have to be quite assertive in getting what I deserve”) were higher than the normal ranges evidenced by other CAPP-SR items in this sample and contributed to the elevation in the sense of entitlement symptom score.
In this study, we endorsed a more literal approach to the translation of CAPP-SR to ensure precision and that the Persian version was equivalent to the English version. However, assertiveness is a multidimensional concept that is varied as a function of social and situational contexts (e.g., Eisler et al.,
1975; Hess et al.,
1980), and is partially impacted by a cognitive filter that individuals interpret social cues (Kirst,
2011; Vagos & Pereira,
2010). Among Iranians, assertiveness is perceived as a valued and adaptive behavior associated with healthy personality adjustment. Thus, it is possible that the essence of this item may not be well articulated to Iranian participants and the rating on this statement might be moderated by the differential perception of assertiveness across cultures.
Consistent with the cross-cultural approach, while there might be certain psychological universals, the form and expression of the psychological processes might be shaped by cultural differences (Cooke,
1998). Therefore, cultural norms not only influence individuals’ evaluations of different behaviors (and items) but also culture forms the way that people (develop) and express psychopathic features by encouraging some behaviors while discouraging others (Cooke & Michie,
1999). This approach is compatible with our findings regarding the Unreliable symptom scale on which Iranian participants scored highly, but did not show any correlation with any of the psychopathy personality scales. This finding is partially consistent with previous findings in Iran that showed Arrogant and Deceitful Interpersonal Style (i.e., Superficial, Deceitful, and Grandiose items; as measured by PCL-SV) could not effectively differentiate Iranian participants with psychopathy from those without (Shariat et al.,
2010). This is also supported by the findings of a nationwide study in Iran, where 65% of participants (n = 17,000) believed that hypocrisy, insincerity, and flattery traits and behaviors have been increasing in that society, and only 18% did not agree with this item (Iranians’ values and attitudes,
2002). In another study, 78% of Iranian respondents (n = 18,045) agreed on the item “Nowadays, people do not really know who the true believer is and who the hypocrite is” (National Research of Iranians’ religiosity,
2011; p. 22 & 132). Iranian scholars view this phenomenon as social harm with historical foundations, and they believe that various cultural, societal, economic, and political factors contribute to maintaining and reinforcing these characteristics (e.g., Anvari,
2018; Nemati,
2013). Because discussing all of these factors is beyond the scope of this paper, we will discuss one of the cultural factors that might explain these characteristics;
ta’ārof.
Ta’ārof—which is not translatable to English—is a fundamental concept in the Persian culture, which is learned very early in life, encompasses broad complex, strategic behaviors and social attitudes, and is practiced in thousands of different ways in everyday discourse (Bahmani,
2004; Beeman,
2020).
Ta’ārof, which represents both linguistic structure and social behavior among Iranians (see Beeman,
2020 for further details), is often equated with politeness, courtesy, and respect to create an atmosphere of trust (Izadi,
2015,
2016). However, it can be viewed as a form of deception, manipulativeness, and insincerity (Campbell,
2006; Fathi,
2004; Nejat,
2004). According to Campbell, “
ta’ārof is also a knowingly false promise based on a false premise” (Campbell,
2006; p. 26). An Iranian communicator knows the rules of
ta’ārof; they are able to read between lines and they understand that if, for example, someone makes promises, that can be merely following cultural norms and that does not mean that the speaker will keep those promises, or someone might withhold the truth for the sake of being polite and considerate of others. This might be perceived as lying by an outsider, but we should also remember that the concept of lying is different across cultures and is affected by sociocultural norms and moral values (Sweetser,
1987).
Importantly, in Iran,
ta’ārof is crucial to successful participation in society and allows individuals not only to be accepted and respected by others but also to achieve their goals (Mahdavi,
2013). Not doing
ta’ārof, on the other hand, can become a source of inefficiency, create social tension, and being labelled as bold and cheeky (Beeman,
2020; Mahdavi,
2013). To support this argument, Iranians have shown to approve of hypocrisy and flattery in society because they believe that this interaction style is “a facilitator,” “a social skill and efficiency,” “a gate to social acceptance and desirability,” “a way to make friendship and relationship,” “a shortcut to success,” and “does not impact one’s sense of commitment and responsibility” (Iran’s Research Center of Art, Culture & Communication,
2014;
http://www.ricac.ac.ir/news.php?item.1074.1).
Thus, unreliability is considered a socially deviant behavior in Iran, but cultural norms have influenced how maladaptive or unethical behavior is perceived, leading to variations in the way unreliability is manifested. This supports our findings that despite being rated high on unreliability, there was no correlation between this symptom and other psychopathy scales. Although unreliability is a significant feature of the psychopathy, its utility may be limited in the conceptual model of psychopathy applied in the Iranian context. Therefore, it should be carefully considered within the Iranian cultural framework.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.