Skip to main content

Welkom bij Scalda & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Scalda heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen.Je kunt de producten hieronder links aanschaffen en rechts inloggen.

Registreer

Schaf de BSL Academy aan: 

BSL Academy mbo AG

Eenmaal aangeschaft kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met Jan van der Velden.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

01-01-2010 | Original Article

Sizing up the associative account of repetition priming

Auteurs: Ian Dennis, Hassina Carder, Timothy J. Perfect

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2010

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Three studies which test an associative account of repetition priming in a size comparison task are reported. Congruence of decision between priming and test affected performance when the priming task and test tasks were the same but not when they differed. This congruence effect was unaffected by the proportion of trials with congruent responses. Same-task priming exceeded cross-task priming even when both tasks required the same aspect of semantic knowledge. The results indicate that a component of priming is due to associations which are formed during priming and automatically activated when stimuli are repeated at test. Stimuli do not become associated with motor responses but are associated with the results of processing at a number of other levels.
Literatuur
go back to reference Bruce, V., Carson, D., Burton, A. M., & Ellis, A. W. (2000). Perceptual priming is not a necessary consequence of semantic classification of pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 289–323. doi:10.1080/027249800390501.CrossRef Bruce, V., Carson, D., Burton, A. M., & Ellis, A. W. (2000). Perceptual priming is not a necessary consequence of semantic classification of pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 289–323. doi:10.​1080/​027249800390501.CrossRef
go back to reference Dobbins, I. G., Schnyer, D. M., Verfaellie, M., & Schacter, D. L. (2004). Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature, 428, 316–319. doi:10.1038/nature02400.CrossRefPubMed Dobbins, I. G., Schnyer, D. M., Verfaellie, M., & Schacter, D. L. (2004). Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature, 428, 316–319. doi:10.​1038/​nature02400.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Franks, J. J., Bilbery, C. W., Guat Lien, K., & McNamara, T. P. (2000). Transfer-appropriate processing (TAP) and repetition priming. Memory & Cognition, 28, 1140–1151. Franks, J. J., Bilbery, C. W., Guat Lien, K., & McNamara, T. P. (2000). Transfer-appropriate processing (TAP) and repetition priming. Memory & Cognition, 28, 1140–1151.
go back to reference Koch, I., & Allport, A. (2006). Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching. Memory & Cognition, 34, 433–444. Koch, I., & Allport, A. (2006). Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching. Memory & Cognition, 34, 433–444.
go back to reference Logan, G. D., & Etherton, G. L. (1994). What is learned during automatization? The role of attention in constructing an instance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1022–1050. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1022.CrossRef Logan, G. D., & Etherton, G. L. (1994). What is learned during automatization? The role of attention in constructing an instance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1022–1050. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​20.​5.​1022.CrossRef
go back to reference Masson, M. E. J., & Macleod, C. M. (1997). Episodic enhancement of processing fluency. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 37). San Diego CA: Academic Press. Masson, M. E. J., & Macleod, C. M. (1997). Episodic enhancement of processing fluency. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 37). San Diego CA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Mosteller, F., & Tukey, J. W. (1977). Data Analysis and Regression: A Second Course in Statistics. Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley. Mosteller, F., & Tukey, J. W. (1977). Data Analysis and Regression: A Second Course in Statistics. Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley.
go back to reference Pollatsek, A., & Well, A. D. (1995). On the use of counterbalanced designs in cognitive research: A suggestion for a better and more powerful analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 785–794. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.3.785.CrossRefPubMed Pollatsek, A., & Well, A. D. (1995). On the use of counterbalanced designs in cognitive research: A suggestion for a better and more powerful analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 785–794. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​21.​3.​785.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & de Houwer, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus–response associations as a source of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 482–495. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.3.482.CrossRef Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & de Houwer, J. (2005). Retrieval of incidental stimulus–response associations as a source of negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 482–495. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​31.​3.​482.CrossRef
go back to reference Scarborough, D. L., Gerard, L., & Cortese, C. (1979). Accessing lexical memory: The transfer of word repetition effects across task and modality. Memory & Cognition, 7, 3–12. Scarborough, D. L., Gerard, L., & Cortese, C. (1979). Accessing lexical memory: The transfer of word repetition effects across task and modality. Memory & Cognition, 7, 3–12.
go back to reference Schacter, D. L., Dobbins, I. G., & Schnyer, D. M. (2004). Specificity of priming: A cognitive neuroscience perspective, Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 5, 853–862.PubMed Schacter, D. L., Dobbins, I. G., & Schnyer, D. M. (2004). Specificity of priming: A cognitive neuroscience perspective, Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 5, 853–862.PubMed
go back to reference Schnyer, D. M., Dobbins, I. G., Nicholls, L., Davis, S., Verfaellie, M., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Item to decision mapping and rapid response learning. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1472–1482. Schnyer, D. M., Dobbins, I. G., Nicholls, L., Davis, S., Verfaellie, M., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Item to decision mapping and rapid response learning. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1472–1482.
go back to reference Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Keane, M. M., & Monti, L. A. (1997). Evidence for multiple mechanisms of conceptual priming on implicit memory tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1324–1343. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.6.1324.CrossRef Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Keane, M. M., & Monti, L. A. (1997). Evidence for multiple mechanisms of conceptual priming on implicit memory tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1324–1343. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​23.​6.​1324.CrossRef
go back to reference Vriezen, E. R., Moscovitch, M., & Bellos, S. A. (1995). Priming of visual and functional knowledge on a semantic classification task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 933–946. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.933.CrossRef Vriezen, E. R., Moscovitch, M., & Bellos, S. A. (1995). Priming of visual and functional knowledge on a semantic classification task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 933–946. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​21.​4.​933.CrossRef
go back to reference Wagenmakers, E.-J. M., Zeelenberg, R., Steyvers, M., Shiffrin, R., & Raajmakers, J. G. W. (2004). Nonword repetition in lexical decision: Support for two opposing processes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1191–1210. Wagenmakers, E.-J. M., Zeelenberg, R., Steyvers, M., Shiffrin, R., & Raajmakers, J. G. W. (2004). Nonword repetition in lexical decision: Support for two opposing processes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1191–1210.
go back to reference Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2005). Interaction of task readiness and automatic retrieval in task switching: Negative priming and competitor priming. Memory & Cognition, 33, 595–610. Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2005). Interaction of task readiness and automatic retrieval in task switching: Negative priming and competitor priming. Memory & Cognition, 33, 595–610.
go back to reference Waszak, F., & Hommel, B. (2007). The costs and benefits of cross-task priming. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1175–1186. Waszak, F., & Hommel, B. (2007). The costs and benefits of cross-task priming. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1175–1186.
go back to reference Zeelenberg, R., Wagenmakers, E.-J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2004). Nonword repetition priming in lexical decision reverses as a function of study task and speed stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 270–277. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.270.CrossRef Zeelenberg, R., Wagenmakers, E.-J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2004). Nonword repetition priming in lexical decision reverses as a function of study task and speed stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 270–277. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​30.​1.​270.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Sizing up the associative account of repetition priming
Auteurs
Ian Dennis
Hassina Carder
Timothy J. Perfect
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2010
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2010
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0224-9