Skip to main content

Welkom bij Scalda & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Scalda heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen.Je kunt de producten hieronder links aanschaffen en rechts inloggen.

Registreer

Schaf de BSL Academy aan: 

BSL Academy mbo AG

Eenmaal aangeschaft kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met Jan van der Velden.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

01-09-2015 | Original Article

The relationship of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity to cognitive processing in adolescents: findings from the ALSPAC birth cohort

Auteurs: Dominika M. Pindus, Robert D. Moore Davis, Charles H. Hillman, Stephan Bandelow, Eef Hogervorst, Stuart J. H. Biddle, Lauren B. Sherar

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The aim of this study was to assess the relations of daily moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) to cognitive functions in 15-year-old adolescents from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children while controlling for aerobic fitness. A sub-sample of 667 adolescents (M age = 15.4 ± 0.16 years; 55 % females) who provided valid data on variables of interest, were used in the analyses. MVPA was objectively assessed using an Actigraph GT1M accelerometer and aerobic fitness was expressed as physical work capacity at the heart rate of 170 beats per minute from a cycle ergometer test. A computerized stop-signal task was used to measure mean reaction time (RT) and standard deviation of RT, as indicators of cognitive processing speed and variability during an attention and inhibitory control task. MVPA was not significantly related to cognitive processing speed or variability of cognitive performance in hierarchical linear regression models. In simple regression models, aerobic fitness was negatively related to mean RT on the simple go condition. Our results suggest that aerobic fitness, but not MVPA, was associated with cognitive processing speed under less cognitively demanding task conditions. The results thus indicate a potential global effect of aerobic fitness on cognitive functions in adolescents but this may differ depending on the specific task characteristics.
Voetnoten
1
In partial correlation analyses with MVPA all cognitive variables except for accuracy were log transformed.
 
2
Due to the limitations of task manipulation and its effects on accuracy measures, our hypotheses focused on the speed and not accuracy of performance. However, we provide further details of the analyses for accuracy measures for interested readers. The analyses of the accuracy data for the stop-signal trials revealed a significant correlation in group one for MVPA (log transformed; pr = 0.12, p = 0.02; accounting for accelerometer wear time). However, this association was not significant in a generalized linear model (B = 0.00, SE = 0.00, Wald’s \(\chi_{(1,\,N\; = \;357)}^{2}\) = 0.04, p = 0.84, LR \(\chi_{(5,\,N\; = \;357)}^{2}\)= 8.23, p = 0.14; accounting for sex, maternal education, aerobic fitness and wear time, ps ≥ 0.18). The associations were tested using generalized linear models for binary and event data with a probit link function. Accuracy data were expressed as the number of responses correct within a set of 32 stop-signal trials. No further significant correlations between MVPA (p = 0.64, group two) or aerobic fitness (ps ≥ 0.06) and accuracy on stop-signal trials were observed.
 
3
The follow-up analyses revealed that aerobic fitness was a significant predictor of accuracy on the go condition in group one (B = -0.17, SE = 0.07, Wald’s \(\chi_{(1,\,N\; = \;357)}^{2}\) = 6.68, p = 0.01, LR \(\chi_{(2,\,N\; = \;357)}^{2}\) = 7.74, p = 0.02; after controlling for sex, p = 0.35). The association was tested using a generalized linear model for binary and event data with a probit link function. Accuracy data were expressed as the number of responses correct within a set of 30 trials.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bassett, D. R., Fitzhugh, E. C., Heath, G. W., Erwin, P. C., Frederick, G. M., Wolff, D. L., et al. (2013). Estimated energy expenditures for school-based policies and active living. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 44(2), 108–113. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2012.10.017.CrossRefPubMed Bassett, D. R., Fitzhugh, E. C., Heath, G. W., Erwin, P. C., Frederick, G. M., Wolff, D. L., et al. (2013). Estimated energy expenditures for school-based policies and active living. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 44(2), 108–113. doi:10.​1016/​j.​amepre.​2012.​10.​017.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Booth, J. N., Tomporowski, P. D., Boyle, J. M., Ness, A. R., Joinson, C., Leary, S. D., et al. (2013). Associations between executive attention and objectively measured physical activity in adolescence: findings from ALSPAC, a UK cohort. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 6(3), 212–219. doi:10.1016/j.mhpa.2013.09.002.CrossRef Booth, J. N., Tomporowski, P. D., Boyle, J. M., Ness, A. R., Joinson, C., Leary, S. D., et al. (2013). Associations between executive attention and objectively measured physical activity in adolescence: findings from ALSPAC, a UK cohort. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 6(3), 212–219. doi:10.​1016/​j.​mhpa.​2013.​09.​002.CrossRef
go back to reference Bouchard, C., Blair, S. N., & Haskell, W. L. (2012). Physical activity and health. Champaign: Human Kinetics. Bouchard, C., Blair, S. N., & Haskell, W. L. (2012). Physical activity and health. Champaign: Human Kinetics.
go back to reference Boyd, A., Golding, J., Macleod, J., Lawlor, D. A., Fraser, A., Henderson, J., et al. (2012). Cohort profile: the ‘Children of the 90 s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 111–127. doi:10.1093/ije/dys064.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Boyd, A., Golding, J., Macleod, J., Lawlor, D. A., Fraser, A., Henderson, J., et al. (2012). Cohort profile: the ‘Children of the 90 s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 111–127. doi:10.​1093/​ije/​dys064.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Braver, T. S., Paxton, J. L., Locke, H. S., & Barch, D. M. (2009). Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(18), 7351–7356. doi:10.1073/pnas.0808187106.CrossRef Braver, T. S., Paxton, J. L., Locke, H. S., & Barch, D. M. (2009). Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(18), 7351–7356. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​0808187106.CrossRef
go back to reference Carnethon, M. R., Gulati, M., & Greenland, P. (2005). Prevalence and cardiovascular disease correlates of low cardiorespiratory fitness in adolescents and adults. Journal of American Medical Association, 294(23), 2981–2988. doi:10.1001/jama.294.23.2981.CrossRef Carnethon, M. R., Gulati, M., & Greenland, P. (2005). Prevalence and cardiovascular disease correlates of low cardiorespiratory fitness in adolescents and adults. Journal of American Medical Association, 294(23), 2981–2988. doi:10.​1001/​jama.​294.​23.​2981.CrossRef
go back to reference Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., VanPatter, M., Pontifex, M. B., et al. (2012a). A functional MRI investigation of the association between childhood aerobic fitness and neurocognitive control. Biological Psychology, 89(1), 260–268. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.017.CrossRefPubMed Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., VanPatter, M., Pontifex, M. B., et al. (2012a). A functional MRI investigation of the association between childhood aerobic fitness and neurocognitive control. Biological Psychology, 89(1), 260–268. doi:10.​1016/​j.​biopsycho.​2011.​10.​017.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chaddock-Heyman, L., Erickson, K.I., Voss, M., Knecht, A., Pontifex, M.B., Castelli, D., et al. (2013). The effects of physical activity on functional MRI activation associated with cognitive control in children: a randomized controlled intervention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–13. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00072. Chaddock-Heyman, L., Erickson, K.I., Voss, M., Knecht, A., Pontifex, M.B., Castelli, D., et al. (2013). The effects of physical activity on functional MRI activation associated with cognitive control in children: a randomized controlled intervention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–13. doi:10.​3389/​fnhum.​2013.​00072.
go back to reference Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., McDowell, J. E., Austin, B. P., Miller, P. H., Yanasak, N. E., et al. (2011). Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychology, 30(1), 91–98. doi:10.1037/a0021766.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., McDowell, J. E., Austin, B. P., Miller, P. H., Yanasak, N. E., et al. (2011). Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychology, 30(1), 91–98. doi:10.​1037/​a0021766.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dencker, M., Thorsson, O., Karlsson, M. K., Lindén, C., Eiberg, S., Wollmer, P., et al. (2007). Gender differences and determinants of aerobic fitness in children aged 8–11 years. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 99(1), 19–26. doi:10.1007/s00421-006-0310-x.CrossRefPubMed Dencker, M., Thorsson, O., Karlsson, M. K., Lindén, C., Eiberg, S., Wollmer, P., et al. (2007). Gender differences and determinants of aerobic fitness in children aged 8–11 years. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 99(1), 19–26. doi:10.​1007/​s00421-006-0310-x.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Department of Health (2011). UK physical activity guidelines. Fact sheet 3: children and young people (5–18 years). In Department of Health (Ed.). London: GOV.UK. Department of Health (2011). UK physical activity guidelines. Fact sheet 3: children and young people (5–18 years). In Department of Health (Ed.). London: GOV.UK.
go back to reference Etnier, J. L. (2006). Are we ready to prescribe physical activity to promote cognitive function? Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 38(5), S29–S30. Etnier, J. L. (2006). Are we ready to prescribe physical activity to promote cognitive function? Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 38(5), S29–S30.
go back to reference Gutin, B., & Owens, S. (2011). The influence of physical activity on cardiometabolic biomarkers in youths: a review. Pediatric Exercise Science, 23(2), 169–185.PubMed Gutin, B., & Owens, S. (2011). The influence of physical activity on cardiometabolic biomarkers in youths: a review. Pediatric Exercise Science, 23(2), 169–185.PubMed
go back to reference Gutman, L. M., & Feinstein, L. (2008). Parenting behaviours and children’s development from infancy to early childhood: changes, continuities and contributions. Early Child Development and Care, 180(4), 535–556. doi:10.1080/03004430802113042.CrossRef Gutman, L. M., & Feinstein, L. (2008). Parenting behaviours and children’s development from infancy to early childhood: changes, continuities and contributions. Early Child Development and Care, 180(4), 535–556. doi:10.​1080/​0300443080211304​2.CrossRef
go back to reference Handley, S. J., Capon, A., Beveriddge, M., Dennis, I., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (2004). Working memory, inhibitory control and the development of children’s reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 10(2), 175–195.CrossRef Handley, S. J., Capon, A., Beveriddge, M., Dennis, I., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (2004). Working memory, inhibitory control and the development of children’s reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 10(2), 175–195.CrossRef
go back to reference Hillman, C. H., Motl, R. W., Pontifex, M. B., Posthuma, D., Stubbe, J. H., Boomsma, D. I., et al. (2006). Physical activity and cognitive function in a cross-section of younger and older community-dwelling individuals. Health Psychology, 25(6), 678–687. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.678.CrossRefPubMed Hillman, C. H., Motl, R. W., Pontifex, M. B., Posthuma, D., Stubbe, J. H., Boomsma, D. I., et al. (2006). Physical activity and cognitive function in a cross-section of younger and older community-dwelling individuals. Health Psychology, 25(6), 678–687. doi:10.​1037/​0278-6133.​25.​6.​678.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lawlor, D., Cooper, A., Bain, C., Davey Smith, G., Irwin, A., Riddoch, C., et al. (2008). Associations of birth size and duration of breast feeding with cardiorespiratory fitness in childhood: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). European Journal of Epidemiology, 23(6), 411–422. doi:10.1007/s10654-008-9259-x.CrossRefPubMed Lawlor, D., Cooper, A., Bain, C., Davey Smith, G., Irwin, A., Riddoch, C., et al. (2008). Associations of birth size and duration of breast feeding with cardiorespiratory fitness in childhood: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). European Journal of Epidemiology, 23(6), 411–422. doi:10.​1007/​s10654-008-9259-x.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego: Academic Press. Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B., & Davis, K. A. (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(2), 276–291. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.10.2.276.PubMed Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B., & Davis, K. A. (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(2), 276–291. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​10.​2.​276.PubMed
go back to reference Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I. H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: the test of everyday attention for children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(8), 1065–1081. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00806.CrossRefPubMed Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I. H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: the test of everyday attention for children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(8), 1065–1081. doi:10.​1111/​1469-7610.​00806.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Anderson, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1998). Test of everyday attention for children (TEA-Ch). Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company. Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Anderson, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1998). Test of everyday attention for children (TEA-Ch). Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.
go back to reference Mattocks, C., Leary, S., Ness, A., Deere, K., Saunders, J., Tilling, K., et al. (2007). Calibration of an accelerometer during free-living activities in children. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity, 2(4), 218–226. doi:10.1080/17477160701408809.CrossRefPubMed Mattocks, C., Leary, S., Ness, A., Deere, K., Saunders, J., Tilling, K., et al. (2007). Calibration of an accelerometer during free-living activities in children. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity, 2(4), 218–226. doi:10.​1080/​1747716070140880​9.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mattocks, C., Ness, A. R., Leary, S. D., Tilling, K., Blair, S. N., Shield, J., et al. (2008). Use of accelerometers in a large field-based study of children: protocols, design issues, and effects on precision. Journal of Physical Activity & Health, 5(Suppl 1), S98–S111. Mattocks, C., Ness, A. R., Leary, S. D., Tilling, K., Blair, S. N., Shield, J., et al. (2008). Use of accelerometers in a large field-based study of children: protocols, design issues, and effects on precision. Journal of Physical Activity & Health, 5(Suppl 1), S98–S111.
go back to reference Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693–2698. doi:10.1073/pnas.1010076108.CrossRef Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693–2698. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​1010076108.CrossRef
go back to reference National Research Council (2013). Educating the student body. Taking physical activity and physical education to school. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. National Research Council (2013). Educating the student body. Taking physical activity and physical education to school. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
go back to reference Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18)., Advances in research and theory New York: Plenum Press.CrossRef Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18)., Advances in research and theory New York: Plenum Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ong, K. K., Emmett, P., Northstone, K., Golding, J., Rogers, I., Ness, A. R., et al. (2009). Infancy weight gain predicts childhood body fat and age at menarche in girls. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 94(5), 1527–1532. doi:10.1210/jc.2008-2489.CrossRefPubMed Ong, K. K., Emmett, P., Northstone, K., Golding, J., Rogers, I., Ness, A. R., et al. (2009). Infancy weight gain predicts childhood body fat and age at menarche in girls. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 94(5), 1527–1532. doi:10.​1210/​jc.​2008-2489.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ortega, F. B., Tresaco, B., Ruiz, J. R., Moreno, L. A., Martin-Matillas, M., Mesa, J. L., et al. (2007). Cardiorespiratory fitness and sedentary activities are associated with adiposity in adolescents. Obesity, 15(6), 1589–1599. doi:10.1038/oby.2007.188.CrossRefPubMed Ortega, F. B., Tresaco, B., Ruiz, J. R., Moreno, L. A., Martin-Matillas, M., Mesa, J. L., et al. (2007). Cardiorespiratory fitness and sedentary activities are associated with adiposity in adolescents. Obesity, 15(6), 1589–1599. doi:10.​1038/​oby.​2007.​188.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Padilla, C., Perez, L., Andres, P., & Parmentier, F. B. R. (2013). Exercise improves cognitive control: evidence from the Stop Signal task. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27(4), 505–511. doi:10.1002/acp.2929.CrossRef Padilla, C., Perez, L., Andres, P., & Parmentier, F. B. R. (2013). Exercise improves cognitive control: evidence from the Stop Signal task. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27(4), 505–511. doi:10.​1002/​acp.​2929.CrossRef
go back to reference Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Johnson, C. R., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(6), 1332–1345. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21528.CrossRefPubMed Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Johnson, C. R., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(6), 1332–1345. doi:10.​1162/​jocn.​2010.​21528.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prince, S., Adamo, K., Hamel, M., Hardt, J., Gorber, S., & Tremblay, M. (2008). A comparison of direct versus self-report measures for assessing physical activity in adults: a systematic review. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 5(1), 56. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-5-56.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Prince, S., Adamo, K., Hamel, M., Hardt, J., Gorber, S., & Tremblay, M. (2008). A comparison of direct versus self-report measures for assessing physical activity in adults: a systematic review. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 5(1), 56. doi:10.​1186/​1479-5868-5-56.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sherar, L., Griew, P., Esliger, D., Cooper, A., Ekelund, U., Judge, K., et al. (2011). International children’s accelerometry database (ICAD): design and methods. BMC Public Health, 11(1), 485.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Sherar, L., Griew, P., Esliger, D., Cooper, A., Ekelund, U., Judge, K., et al. (2011). International children’s accelerometry database (ICAD): design and methods. BMC Public Health, 11(1), 485.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 745–759. doi:10.1080/17470210500162854.CrossRefPubMed St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 745–759. doi:10.​1080/​1747021050016285​4.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference The US Department of Health and Human Services (2008). 2008 physical activity guidelines for Americans: Be active, healthy, and happy (Vol. Chapter 3, pp. 13–19). Washington. The US Department of Health and Human Services (2008). 2008 physical activity guidelines for Americans: Be active, healthy, and happy (Vol. Chapter 3, pp. 13–19). Washington.
go back to reference Tremblay, M. S., Shields, M., Laviolette, M., Craig, C. L., Janssen, I., & Connor Gorber, S. (2010). Fitness of Canadian children and youth: results from the 2007–2009 Canadian Health Measures Survey. Health Reports, 21(1), 7–20.PubMed Tremblay, M. S., Shields, M., Laviolette, M., Craig, C. L., Janssen, I., & Connor Gorber, S. (2010). Fitness of Canadian children and youth: results from the 2007–2009 Canadian Health Measures Survey. Health Reports, 21(1), 7–20.PubMed
go back to reference West, R., Murphy, K. J., Armilio, M. L., Craik, F. I. M., & Stuss, D. T. (2002). Lapses of intention and performance variability reveal age-related increases in fluctuations of executive control. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 402–419. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1507.CrossRefPubMed West, R., Murphy, K. J., Armilio, M. L., Craik, F. I. M., & Stuss, D. T. (2002). Lapses of intention and performance variability reveal age-related increases in fluctuations of executive control. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 402–419. doi:10.​1006/​brcg.​2001.​1507.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wu, C.-T., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Kramer, A. F., et al. (2011). Aerobic fitness and response variability in preadolescent children performing a cognitive control task. Neuropsychology, 25(3), 333–341. doi:10.1037/a0022167.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Wu, C.-T., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Kramer, A. F., et al. (2011). Aerobic fitness and response variability in preadolescent children performing a cognitive control task. Neuropsychology, 25(3), 333–341. doi:10.​1037/​a0022167.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
The relationship of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity to cognitive processing in adolescents: findings from the ALSPAC birth cohort
Auteurs
Dominika M. Pindus
Robert D. Moore Davis
Charles H. Hillman
Stephan Bandelow
Eef Hogervorst
Stuart J. H. Biddle
Lauren B. Sherar
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2015
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0612-2