Skip to main content

Welkom bij Scalda & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Scalda heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen.Je kunt de producten hieronder links aanschaffen en rechts inloggen.

Registreer

Schaf de BSL Academy aan: 

BSL Academy mbo AG

Eenmaal aangeschaft kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met Jan van der Velden.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top

Open Access 18-02-2025 | Original Paper

The Role of Emotion Regulation in Distinct Measures of Emotional Resilience

Auteurs: Hannah A. Razak, Colin MacLeod, Daniel Rudaizky, Lies Notebaert

Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Background

Research suggests that emotion regulation plays a critical role in emotional resilience, however due to inconsistencies with how emotional resilience has been operationalised, the exact nature of this relationship remains relatively unexplored. We examined two hypotheses across three distinct operationalisations: outcome-based, transient, and trait-based, recognising resilience as an outcome, a transient dynamic construct, and a perceived trait, respectively. Specifically, whether (1) a greater tendency to choose reappraisal relative to distraction or (2) a greater tendency to choose reappraisal aligned with the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon (i.e., reappraisal for low-intensity stimuli and distraction for high-intensity stimuli), was associated with greater emotional resilience, and whether these relationships were partially accounted for by effective downregulation.

Methods

Young undergraduate adults (final n = 113) attended an experimental session where each measure of emotional resilience was obtained. The outcome-based was measured using a residual approach, the transient measure through the degree of emotional recovery following exposure to a standardised stressor task in the lab and the trait-based measure using the Brief Resilience Scale. In a second session, participants viewed high and low intensity images and chose between reappraisal and distraction to downregulate negative emotions elicited by these stimuli. In some trials, participants were instructed to use either strategy. The effectiveness of these downregulation attempts was measured.

Results

A greater tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction, was associated with greater transient and trait-based measures, but not with the outcome-based measure. Reappraisal aligned to the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon was not related to emotional resilience.

Conclusions

Our findings are consistent with theory stating that tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction may contribute to emotional resilience. However, the differential associations evident suggest different mechanisms may relate to distinct operationalisations of this construct. Critically, the cross-sectional design of the current study limits inferences of causality and directionality. Future work replicating and extending on these findings across the distinct operationalisations are warranted.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10608-025-10581-6.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Young adults are at increased susceptibility to the onset of mental health disorders, with the prevalence of anxiety-related, depression-related and substance-use related disorders, ranked among the top 20 disabilities experienced within this age group worldwide (Gustavson et al., 2018; Kieling et al., 2024). The onset of these mental health disorders can have detrimental impacts at both an individual and societal level, linked to issues such as increased risk of future mental health disorders, chronic unemployment, incarceration, homelessness, and suicide (Catalano & Kellogg, 2020). Research has repeatedly shown adversity as a key antecedent factor to the onset of emotional difficulties and subsequent mental health problems (Juwariah et al., 2022) and unfortunately exposure to adverse experiences is common, occurring throughout the human lifespan (Rosenman & Rodgers, 2004). Yet, there are large individual differences in the degree to which individuals emotionally recover following such exposure (Bonanno, 2004). This individual difference dimension is characterised by a key construct referred to as emotional resilience.
Emotional resilience has been conceptualised in many different ways; however almost all conceptualisations reference two main components. The first is exposure to adversity and the second is demonstration of some form of positive emotional adaptation following such exposure (Kalisch et al., 2017; Southwick et al., 2014). Whilst research on emotional resilience has increased, progress in understanding the cognitive mechanisms which contribute to individual differences in this construct has been lacking, mainly due to this lack of conceptual clarity resulting in large disparities in its operational definition across studies (Bonanno et al., 2015; Hiebel et al., 2021; Kalisch et al., 2015; Luthar et al., 2000). Despite these inconsistencies, studies have outlined the utility of further understanding this construct, with findings that young adults demonstrating high levels of emotional resilience, even when measured to reflect various conceptualisations, were consistently shown to have better mental, physical and occupational outcomes (Kong et al., 2015) including lower levels of psychological distress (Haddadi & Ali Besharat, 2010), better physical health (Nath & Pradhan, 2012) and increased likelihood of being in employment, education or training despite adversity exposure and other risk factors (Cahill et al., 2022). We will begin this section by first providing an overview of the distinct conceptualisations of emotional resilience commonly adopted in the literature and its measurement, followed by an overview of theoretical frameworks and experimental findings supporting the role of emotion regulation as a potential cognitive mechanism underpinning individual differences in this construct.

Conceptualisations of Emotional Resilience

Research conducted on emotional resilience has typically either conceptualised the construct as a trait or as an outcome of a dynamic process which is reflected in the different approaches to its measurement. First, earlier conceptualisations in the literature have defined emotional resilience as a stable trait or disposition that promotes positive adaptation against the negative emotional impacts of experiencing adverse events (Block & Kremen, 1996; Connor & Davidson, 2003). Research designs following this trait-based conceptualisation often use self-report measures to examine resilience, whereby scales with the best psychometric ratings included items typically focused on the measurement of protective factors thought to contribute to recovery from adversity (e.g., personal competence, acceptance, social support) or on one’s ability to ‘bounce back’ from stress (Windle et al., 2011). However, many theorists have criticised this conceptualisation and the way that it has been measured. Firstly, the static nature of trait-based conceptualisations limits the possibility that emotional resilience may vary moment-to-moment, across situations and further develop throughout the life span (Cahill et al., 2022; Rutter, 2012). Secondly, there appears to be a lack of ‘gold standard’ across the use of self-report measures raising concerns on the extent to which these scales are indeed measuring the true intended construct (Kalisch et al., 2017; Windle et al., 2011). Specifically, despite exposure to adversity being a core component to the conceptualisation of emotional resilience, many self-report scales do not adequately account for the variation in levels of adversity an individual may have experienced. Thus, it has been increasingly argued that these scales may be more reflective of one’s perception of their own resilience as opposed to a measure of the true intended construct (Britt et al., 2021; Notebaert et al., 2024).
To account for these concerns, an emerging consensus within the literature have begun to conceptualise emotional resilience as an outcome that is achieved through a dynamic process involving successful adaptation to adversity that is dependent on one’s context and resources rather than being a fixed attribute (Cicchetti & Blender, 2006; Kalisch et al., 2021; Rutter, 2012). Accordingly, this perspective has operationalised emotional resilience as the demonstration of more positive emotional outcomes than what is to be expected relative to the adversity experienced (Kalisch et al., 2021; Parsons et al., 2016).
Research designs following this operationalisation have indexed emotional resilience through a well-validated statistical approach, known as the residual approach (Booth et al., 2022; Cahill et al., 2022; Höltge & Ungar, 2022). This approach involves regressing a measure of emotional outcome onto a measure of experienced adversity and saving the residual scores as a proxy for each individual’s level of emotional resilience. These scores represent how much better or worse an individual has emotionally recovered relative to what would be predicted based on the adversity experienced. As positive emotional outcomes can manifest across various ways following experiences of adversity, this approach assumes a significant relationship between adversity type and post-adversity emotional outcome (Cahill et al., 2022). Specifically, according to a conceptual framework proposed by Kalisch et al. (2015), the selected measure must be sensitive to the expected changes in emotional outcomes for the type of adversity assessed within the given sample (see also Kalisch et al., 2021). For example, in line with this framework, researchers have indexed this outcome-based operationalisation in a sample of young adults by defining positive emotional outcomes as the absence of emotional difficulties following experiences of negative life events (Notebaert et al., 2024).
Moreover, in recognition of its dynamic nature, theorists have emphasised distinguishing between operationally defining emotional resilience as a distal outcome arising from positive emotional adaptation to multiple adversities over time (as per the residual approach), and emotional resilience as a proximal outcome following a single incident of acute stressor exposure (Bonanno & Diminich, 2013). For example, while transient perturbations in emotional functioning are expected during and immediately following an isolated event, individuals demonstrating high levels of emotional resilience demonstrate minimal to no lasting effects (Bonanno et al., 2015; Degering et al., 2023). Despite this recognition, research examining individual differences based on this operationalisation is lacking. Current literature has typically operationalised the construct in one particular way, either as a perceived trait via self-report scales or as an outcome of a process via the residual approach. This narrow approach limits the ability to compare candidate mechanisms across studies and across the different ways emotional resilience may be manifested (Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000; Luthar et al., 2000). Thus, to build a more comprehensive understanding of the cognitive factors underpinning individual differences in emotional resilience, future research must engage with and systematically compare these distinct operationalisations.

Emotion Regulation as a Candidate Cognitive Mechanism

The cognitive process of emotion regulation has been proposed as one candidate mechanism underpinning why some individuals show more emotional resilience than others (Troy & Mauss, 2011). Emotion regulation is conceptualised as processes through which individuals modulate their emotions in response to environmental demands by deploying regulatory strategies to either modify the type of, or magnitude of, their emotional experience or of the emotion-eliciting event itself (Aldao et al., 2010). Examples of effective emotion regulation can be seen when an individual successfully reduces the intensity of their negative emotional experience through a process known as downregulation, or to increase the intensity of their positive emotional experience through a process known as upregulation (Gross, 2002, 2015).
Theoretical models conceptualise emotion regulation as a process that can be divided into a series of stages following exposure to an emotion-eliciting event (Bonanno & Burton, 2013; Gross, 2015; Sheppes et al., 2015). First, the need to emotionally regulate is identified, then a specific emotion regulation strategy is selected, implemented and its success monitored (Sheppes, 2020). Critically, as will be expanded below, individuals differ in their tendency to select specific emotion regulation strategies and the effectiveness of regulatory implementation, which has been described as two complementary, but discrete stages of emotion regulation (Rammensee et al., 2023). Given that adverse events are inherently emotional, an individual’s ability to effectively regulate their negative emotions during implementation may be a critical mechanism in supporting emotional well-being and enhanced emotional coping when exposed to stressful events (Kobylińska & Kusev, 2019). Conversely, the inability to effectively regulate emotions has been proposed to increase vulnerability to prolonged intense distress that can devolve into various psychopathologies (Aldao et al., 2010; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). A recent systematic review by Polizzi and Steven (2021) found substantial support across studies supporting a positive association between effective emotion regulation and emotional resilience. However, limitations relating to the use of self-report scales across studies for measuring emotional resilience and to infer effective emotion regulation were evident which warrant several considerations.
Firstly, whilst this review was comprehensive in examining articles defining emotional resilience across the different conceptualisations commonly used in the literature, majority of the studies included indexed emotional resilience using self-report questionnaires without measuring exposure to adversity, which as outlined above, may only provide evidence for operationalisations of this construct as a perceived trait. Secondly, most studies which directly assessed effective emotion regulation in this review also adopted this same approach (e.g., Banyard et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2019; Mestre et al., 2017; Polizzi et al., 2018; Poole et al., 2017; Powers et al., 2015; Vaughan et al., 2019). Self-report scales to infer effective emotion regulation tend to collapse different aspects of emotion regulation, such as perceived difficulties to successfully upregulate positive and/or downregulate negative emotions (e.g., DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004) and the perceived frequency of using strategies deemed as adaptive or maladaptive (e.g., ERQ; Gross & John, 2003), into a single global measure. However, as the effectiveness of emotion regulation can vary depending on the strategy deployed and the nature of the emotion-eliciting event (Gross, 2015), a critical issue with this approach is that individuals retrospectively report their behaviour without taking into account the decision-making process involved in strategy selection and the specific emotion-eliciting events being referred to when answering these questions (Sheppes, 2020).
Indeed, self-report questionnaires to examine emotional experience retrospectively have been associated with a range of biases (Barrett, 1997; Levine & Safer, 2002). Behavioural tasks that involve exposing participants to the same emotion-eliciting event and examining the difference in emotional experience before and after deploying a regulatory strategy may provide a more accurate indicator of effective emotion regulation (Sheppes, 2020; Troy et al., 2010). Despite this, studies adopting this approach to examine the relationship between effective emotion regulation and emotional resilience are lacking. The few studies that have examined effective emotion regulation using behavioural tasks have only examined its relationship with trait-based emotional resilience measures (e.g., Hildebrandt et al., 2016; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004). Thus, whilst the review found substantial support for the relationship between effective emotion regulation and trait-based emotional resilience, more rigorous behavioural testing examining the relationship between effective emotion regulation and across the distinct operationalisations of emotional resilience is needed.
Now, we will consider the role of different emotion regulation strategies in relation to its effectiveness, and how the tendency to choose certain strategies may differentially relate to emotional resilience. As adverse life events can occur from a position where individuals are unable to change or avoid the negative emotion-eliciting event (e.g., death of a family member), cognitive strategies such as reappraisal and distraction that involve a change in emotional response to the situation, rather than a change to the situation itself, may be particularly useful in the context of adversity exposure. Reappraisal involves engaging with negative emotion by reinterpreting the meaning of an emotion-eliciting event to be more neutral or positive, whilst distraction involves disengaging from negative emotion by producing neutral thoughts unrelated to the emotion-eliciting event (Sheppes et al., 2011). However, as will be argued, these regulation strategies differ in their effectiveness for downregulation, which may influence how it is related to emotional resilience.
Historically, reappraisal has been considered the most effective emotion regulation strategy (Webb et al., 2012). As reappraisal involves reinterpreting the meaning of a negative emotional-eliciting event, such active engagement alters its subsequent emotional impact, and facilitates processing, evaluating, and remembering emotional information, all of which are essential for long-term coping (Folkman et al., 1986; Gross, 2002; Sheppes et al., 2014a, b). Empirical and meta-analytic evidence supports this, showing that reappraisal is associated with successful downregulation of negative emotions (Augustine & Hemenover, 2009; Wang et al., 2021; Webb et al., 2012), better mental health outcomes (Aldao et al., 2010; Gross & John, 2003) and greater levels of trait-based emotional resilience (Mestre, et al., 2017).
Additionally, the positive appraisal style theory of resilience (PASTOR; Kalisch et al., 2015), identifies reappraisal as a key mechanism underpinning emotionally resilient outcomes, as the ability to properly reappraise aversive situations involving appropriate adjustments of the initial negative appraisal and/or the generation of complementary positive appraisals, attenuates ongoing stress responses. Research has shown that tendency to use reappraisal can buffer the negative effects of adversity exposure. For example, in a sample of young adults, self-reported habitual use of cognitive reappraisal was associated with lower post traumatic and internalising symptoms following exposure to intense media coverage of a local terror attack and problems transitioning into university, respectively (Jenness et al., 2016; Zahniser & Conley, 2018). Together, these findings provide sound theoretical reasoning that greater reappraisal use is indeed associated with greater emotional resilience (Kalisch et al., 2015).
Conversely, while distraction can be effective for the downregulation of negative emotions, there is evidence suggesting that repeated use of distraction diminishes its efficacy (Kross & Ayduk, 2008; Paul et al., 2016; Thiruchselvam et al., 2011). Specifically, as distraction involves replacing negative emotional content in working memory with neutral content retrieved from long-term memory, such high cognitive cost can impair emotional processing of the triggering event, and lead to poorer future recall of the event (Richards & Gross, 2006; Sheppes & Meiran, 2007). As distraction does not address how emotional experiences are dealt with in the future, a habitual tendency to use distraction may thus be less effective than reappraisal for downregulation and more detrimental for long-term coping. For example, one study found that compared to instructions to simply attend to aversive stimuli, using distraction resulted in a significant increase in self-reported unpleasantness when participants were re-exposed to the same adversity within a single experimental session (Paul et al., 2016). Additionally, studies have demonstrated greater instructed and self-reported distraction use during stressful periods were significantly associated with a greater likelihood of developing depression in the future (Holahan et al., 2005; Kross & Ayduk, 2008).
Thus, given the effectiveness of reappraisal across situations and its benefits for long-term coping, one possible hypothesis is the Reappraisal Dominance Hypothesis. This hypothesis proposes that a greater tendency to choose reappraisal, relative to distraction, contributes to greater emotional resilience as reappraisal use leads to more effective negative emotion downregulation than distraction. One prediction arising from this hypothesis is that in a cross-sectional design, there will be an association between tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction and emotional resilience, which would be in part accounted for by effective downregulation. Thus, the first aim of the present study is to test this prediction across the distinct operationalisations of emotional resilience.
However, it is also possible that the proposed relationship between reappraisal choice tendencies and emotional resilience is more complex. The cognitive model of psychological resilience proposed by Parsons et al. (2016) posits that individuals showing emotionally resilient outcomes flexibly apply different cognitive processing strategies that are most conducive to current situational demands in achieving a desired goal. For instance, if applying a specific cognitive processing strategy in response to an adverse event does not promote the goal of effective coping with the elicited stress, these individuals will then adaptively change strategy to one that is more in line with achieving this goal. This perspective on the cognitive basis of emotional resilience is in line with more recent research conducted on emotion regulation flexibility, referred to as the ability to implement emotion regulation strategies that synchronize with contextual demands (Aldao et al., 2015). Specifically, studies by Sheppes and colleagues (2011, Sheppes et al., 2014a, b) examining emotion regulation choice in response to negative situations that varied in intensity demonstrated the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon whereby individuals tended to choose reappraisal for low intensity negative situations and distraction for high intensity negative situations.
The emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon has been consistently replicated in the literature (Shafir et al., 2015, 2016; Van Bockstaele et al., 2020) and its effectiveness demonstrated in studies whereby instructed distraction use, relative to instructed reappraisal use, resulted in more effective downregulation of negative emotions for high intensity stimuli and equally effective for low intensity stimuli (Shafir et al., 2015; Sheppes et al., 2014a, b). Despite both strategies demonstrating equal levels of effectiveness in negative emotion downregulation for low intensity stimuli, choosing to deploy distraction relative to reappraisal in this context is suggested to be detrimental in promoting long-term mental health as it does not facilitate emotional challenges being dealt with in the future. So, choosing reappraisal for low intensity situations and distraction for high intensity situations, as is evident in the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon, may be the most effective pattern of emotion regulation strategy choice.
Based on this evidence, another possible hypothesis for the role of reappraisal choice tendencies underpinning emotional resilience is the Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis. This hypothesis proposes that greater reappraisal tendency alignment (i.e., choosing reappraisal for low intensity and distraction for high intensity), hereon referred to as reappraisal tendency alignment, contributes to greater emotional resilience, as this pattern of emotion regulation choice leads to more effective negative emotion downregulation. One prediction arising from this hypothesis is that in a cross-sectional design, there will be an association between reappraisal tendency alignment and emotional resilience, which would be explained in part by effective downregulation. Thus, the second aim of the present study is to examine this prediction across the distinct operationalisations of emotional resilience.
It is currently unclear whether a greater tendency to choose reappraisal relative to distraction in general, underpins variation in emotional resilience as this would lead to more effective downregulation, or whether greater reappraisal tendency alignment, underpins variation in emotional resilience, as this pattern of reappraisal tendency choice leads to more effective downregulation. Much of the current research within the emotion regulation literature has focused primarily on the effects of self-reported habitual use of reappraisal or distraction on mental health outcomes (Aldao et al., 2010; Holahan et al., 2005; Kross & Ayduk, 2008), or on people’s tendency to choose one strategy over the other when exposed to varying emotional contexts (). However, despite suggestions made regarding the critical role of reappraisal as a protective factor against negative emotional outcomes under stressor exposure (Kalisch et al., 2015; Riepenhausen et al., 2022) and a growing consensus that flexible emotion regulation strategy choice is crucial in promoting adaptive coping following stressor exposure and better mental health outcomes (Aldao et al., 2015; Bonanno & Burton, 2013; Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010), the relationship of either possibility underpinning variation across the distinct operationalisations of emotion resilience remains untested.

The Current Study

In summary, the current study was designed to examine associations between reappraisal choice tendencies and individual differences in emotional resilience, to test the cross-sectional predictions generated by two key hypotheses. The Reappraisal Dominance Hypothesis proposes that a greater tendency to choose reappraisal relative to distraction, contributes to greater emotional resilience as reappraisal use leads to more effective downregulation than distraction. Second, the Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis proposes greater tendency to choose reappraisal aligned to the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon contributes to greater emotional resilience, as this pattern of emotion regulation choice leads to more effective downregulation.
To examine these hypotheses, the current study design focuses on a sample of emerging adults attending university as this sample reflects a critical transitional stage of development. This stage includes many positive but also stressful challenges involved in establishing future trajectories in relation to educational, occupational, and social attainments uniquely central to this cohort (Arnett, 2003, 2007; Sandhu, 1994). Furthermore, as most mental health disorders emerge by emerging adulthood, this sample reflects a crucial time for effective interventions in preventing risks and promoting positive development (Arnett, et al., 2014; Catalano & Kellogg, 2020). To assess emotional resilience measured to reflect distinct operationalisations, indices of past adversity, current emotional difficulties and perceived trait resilience were obtained, followed by exposure to a standardised lab-based adverse event. This allowed for the derivation of three different indices of emotional resilience: a Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS) Residual Index reflecting outcome-based operationalisations consisting of less emotional difficulties than expected relative to past adversity experienced (Notebaert et al., 2024), a Brief Resilience Scale (BRS) Index reflecting trait-based conceptualisations of perceived emotional resilience (Windle et al., 2011), and a Lab Based Stressor Recovery Index reflecting a transient measure of emotional resilience (Bonanno & Diminich, 2013) (see Methods). Participants completed an Emotion Regulation Assessment Task in which they were exposed to stimuli of high and low intensity and chose either reappraisal or distraction to downregulate negative emotions elicited by these stimuli (Sheppes et al., 2011). Participants also rated the intensity of their negative emotions across each trial, which rendered a measure of the effectiveness of their downregulation attempts.
The Reappraisal Dominance Hypothesis generated the following predictions. First, the tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction will be positively associated with emotional resilience. Secondly, downregulation effectiveness will partially account for the statistical relationship between the tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction and emotional resilience. Conversely, the Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis generated the following predictions. First, the tendency to choose reappraisal aligned to the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon (i.e., reappraisal tendency alignment) will be positively associated with emotional resilience. Secondly, downregulation effectiveness will partially account for the statistical relationship between reappraisal tendency alignment and emotional resilience. To comprehensively examine each hypothesis, these predictions were tested on each measure of emotional resilience reflecting distinct operational definitions1 entered as the dependent variable.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from a pool of undergraduate Psychology students from The University of Western Australia and Curtin University in exchange for course credit. A total of 205 participants were recruited, however 12 participants did not complete the study and a further 92 participants were excluded from tests of hypotheses (see Results) for not meeting the experimental requirements. The final sample included 113 participants, with 80 identifying as female, 32 as male and 1 as non-binary, and an age range of 17 to 26 (M = 19.95, SD = 1.96). Participants’ ethnicity within this sample included 50% Oceanian, 19% South-East Asian, 13% North-West European, 7% Southern and Central Asian, 5% North-East Asian, 3% North African and Middle Eastern, 2% Sub-Saharan African and 1% Southern and Eastern European.
Following the inverse square root method for assessing statistical power (Kock & Hadaya, 2018), this sample size was deemed sufficient to detect the predicted direct and indirect associations between reappraisal choice tendencies and emotional resilience (Nmin = 69). Due to the novelty of the effects under test and an aim to detect effects of practical significance, this calculation was made based on the upper boundary of a moderate effect size (βmin = .30) with 80% power at a significance level of 0.05.

Materials

Assessment of Emotional Resilience Operationalised as an Outcome and a Trait

To derive a Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS) Residual Index using the residual approach, such that it reflects a measure of the outcome-based operationalisation of emotional resilience, self-report questionnaires measuring past adversity and emotional difficulties were included. Additionally, to derive a Brief Resilience Scale (BRS) Index, such that it reflects a measure of the trait-based operationalisation of perceived emotional resilience, a traditional resilience self-report scale was also included.
To measure exposure to past adversity, the 25-item Negative Life Events for Students Scale (NLESS; Buri et al., 2015) was used. Adversity was operationalised as the number of negative life events with the potential to elicit emotional difficulties experienced in the past year (Kalisch et al., 2021). The NLESS contains items relating to negative life events relevant to the student population, such as parental divorce and death of a close friend. For each item, participants indicated whether they had experienced each listed event in the past 12 months with the total number of ‘yes’ responses summed to derive a total NLESS score, where higher scores reflect a greater number of experienced adverse events.
To measure emotional difficulties,2 the 21-item Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) was used. The DASS-21 was included as it assesses symptoms of emotional difficulties across the three domains of depression, anxiety, and stress, with seven items mapping onto each dimension. This scale was chosen as negative life events experienced in the past year has been shown to predict an increased risk for developing depression, anxiety and stress disorders (McLaughlin et al., 2010), thus meeting a core assumption of the residual approach (Kalisch et al., 2021). Additionally, due to its transdiagnostic nature, the DASS-21 is a more suitable instrument for indexing the outcome-based measure of emotional resilience compared to other instruments that focus on symptoms of only one dimension of emotional experience (Kalisch et al., 2015). Participants rated on a 4-point scale, ranging from 0 (did not apply to me at all) to 3 (applied to me most of the time) on the extent to which they had experienced each symptom within the last two weeks. To derive a total DASS score each item was summed, where higher scores reflect greater emotional difficulties. Each sub-scale of the DASS-21 has good internal consistency reliability and good convergent validity within an Australian student sample (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). In the current sample, Cronbach’s α for the depression, anxiety, and stress subscale as well as the composite score demonstrated good internal consistency reliability with .89, .85, .83 and .93, respectively.
To gain a trait-based measure of perceived emotional resilience, the 6-item Brief Resilience Scale (BRS; Smith et al., 2008) was used as it assesses an individual’s perceived ability to bounce back from adversity. This scale was selected as it is the only scale assessing trait operationalisations of emotional resilience which references exposure to adversity in each item (Windle et al., 2011). Participants rate on a 5-point scale the extent to which they agree to each item, ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 5 (Strongly agree). Half of the items are positively worded (i.e., “It does not take me long to recover from a stressful event”) and the other half are negatively worded (i.e., “I tend to take a long time to get over setbacks in my life”). A BRS index was then calculated by first reverse coding negatively worded items and then obtaining the average score across all items, where higher scores reflect greater emotional resilience perceived as a trait. The BRS has good construct validity and internal consistency reliability in a student sample (Smith et al., 2008), as well as demonstrated good internal consistency reliability in the current sample (α = .85).

Assessment of Emotional Resilience as a Transient Outcome of a Dynamic Construct

To capture operationalisations of resilience that emphasise its dynamic nature (Rutter, 2012), we include an assessment reflecting a transient measure of emotional resilience. This was done through the delivery of a standardized stressor task in the lab. We measured state anxiety in the lead up and aftermath of this stressor to render a Lab Stressor Recovery Index.
To obtain the measures required for the Lab Stressor Recovery Index, exposure to a standardized stressor task with the potential to elicit negative emotions was first required. The current study utilised an anagram stressor task with bogus poor performance feedback adapted from MacLeod et al. (2002). In this task, participants were required to solve as many anagrams as possible within a set timeframe of three minutes whilst being video recorded. Participants were told that recordings of students performing very poorly or very highly would be used in future Psychology classes as course content on behavioural and physiological markers of task performance. Critically, most anagrams were difficult or impossible to solve, and participants received on-screen feedback reflective of their poor performance. Further details on this task can be found in the Supplementary Materials.
To permit calculation of the Lab Stressor Recovery Index, a measure of fluctuations in state anxiety-related emotions in response to the standardised stressor task was required. The 6-item Spielberger State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-6; Marteau & Bekker, 1992) assesses state anxiety-related emotions, thus was used for this purpose. The STAI-6 was administered across four time points: immediately before receiving instructions to the standardised stressor task (pre-adversity), immediately after completing the test period (post-adversity), mid-way through the breathing task (mid-recovery) and at the very end of the breathing task (end-recovery). This scale was chosen as it has good internal consistency reliability and is sensitive to different levels of stressors and fluctuations in state anxiety (Marteau & Bekker, 1992), thus provides an appropriate measure in capturing anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration in response to the standardised stressor task.
The STAI-6 includes three anxiety-present (e.g. tense) and three anxiety-absent (e.g. calm) items. For each item, participants indicated to each item how they felt ‘right now’ using a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much so). The anxiety-absent items were reverse scored and summed with the anxiety-present items to obtain a state-anxiety score for each participant calculated separately for each assessment point. Higher scores reflect heightened state-anxiety. The internal consistency reliability of the STAI-6 was good at pre-adversity, post-adversity, mid-recovery and end-recovery time point (α = .82, .85, .85, .85, respectively).

Apparatus

All participants completed this experiment in person using software Inquisit 6 (v.6.6.1.; 2022) on a Windows PC with a 17-in. monitor screen, set at a resolution of 1920 × 1080. Responses were recorded using a standard mouse and QWERTY keyboard.

Emotion Regulation Assessment Task

To permit hypotheses testing regarding the relationship between emotion regulation strategy choices and emotional resilience, an adapted version of Sheppes et al. (2011) Emotion Regulation Choice task was designed, referred to here as the Emotion Regulation Assessment task. This task included a Choice block to derive a measure of Reappraisal Tendency, Reappraisal Tendency Alignment, and Downregulation Effectiveness. An additional block where participants were instructed to use either strategy was also included as we considered that each hypothesis may only be valid for individuals for whom implementing each strategy in general and/or for specific stimuli indeed led to effective downregulation. More details on this aspect of the task and the exploratory analyses can be found in the Supplementary Materials. Practice trials were included to ensure participants understood task requirements including instructions for how to implement each strategy as per experiment one in Sheppes et al. (2011) study. The stimulus set used in this task included negative visual images obtained from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Bradley et al., 1997). The stimulus set was divided into two subsets which included an equal number of negative visual images rated high in intensity and low in intensity. Further details regarding these images can be found in the Online Supplementary Materials.
Each trial of the Choice block included five chronological steps: pre-regulation view, pre-regulation rating, strategy choice, downregulating view, and post-regulation rating (see Fig. 1). In the pre-regulation view phase, the word “watch” was displayed in the centre of the black screen for 2000 ms and then replaced with a high or low intensity image. Participants were required to view the presented image for 2000 ms before the image disappeared. In the pre-regulation rating phase, participants responded on a 9-point scale (1 = not intense at all to 9 = very intense) to the statement “Please indicate how intense you would rate your negative emotions (fear, threat, disgust, sadness, anger…) while viewing the image.” Next, in the strategy choice phase, participants were required to choose between reappraisal and distraction to downregulate their negative emotions elicited by the image that was just presented, indicating their selected choice using the mouse. Once chosen, the word “Reappraise” or “Distract” appeared for 2000 ms depending on which choice was made. Participants then moved on to the downregulating view phase where they deployed their chosen strategy to downregulate while viewing the same image that was presented in the pre-regulation view phase for 8000 ms. Finally, in the post-regulation rating phase, participants were required to rate the intensity of their negative emotions again using the same statement and response scale as described for the pre-regulation rating phase.
The Choice block included 20 trials in total containing an equal number of trials presenting high intensity images and low intensity images. For each participant, the images presented were randomly selected from the two stimulus subsets of high and low intensity images. Image presentations were counterbalanced across participants to ensure differences in strategy choice and effectiveness at downregulation were not due to the use of specific images.
The Choice block allowed for the derivation of three critical measures for each participant. Firstly, Reappraisal Tendency was calculated by dividing the number of trials reappraisal was chosen by the total number of trials in this block. Greater Reappraisal Tendency scores reflects a higher tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction regardless of image intensity. Secondly, Reappraisal Tendency Alignment was calculated by summing the number of trials reappraisal was chosen for low intensity images with the number of trials distraction was chosen for high intensity images and dividing this sum by the total number of trials in this block. Greater Reappraisal Tendency Alignment scores reflect a greater extent to which participants’ emotion regulation strategy choices were consistent with the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon. Finally, Downregulation Effectiveness was calculated by subtracting the average rating of negative emotion intensity across the post-regulation rating phase from the average rating of negative emotion intensity across the pre-regulation rating phase. Greater Downregulation Effectiveness scores reflect stronger reduction of negative emotion intensity across the Choice block regardless of the chosen strategy and image intensity.
The Instruct block was divided into two sub-blocks, a reappraisal sub-block and distraction sub-block. Each trial in the Instruct block was identical in steps to the trials described in the Choice block with only one critical difference. In the Instruct block, participants were not provided with a choice between strategies, instead the strategy choice phase in the Choice block was replaced with an instructed strategy phase. In the instructed strategy phase, participants were presented with one word in the centre of the screen of either “Reappraise” or “Distract” for 2000 ms. Each trial in the reappraisal sub-block presented the word “Reappraise,” whilst each trial in the distraction sub-block presented the word “Distract.” Participants were then required to use the instructed strategy from this phase to downregulate from their negative emotions whilst viewing the presented image in the downregulating view phase.
The Instruct block consisted of 40 trials in total, including 20 trials in the reappraisal sub-block and 20 trials in the distraction sub-block. In each sub-block, 10 trials presented high intensity images, and 10 trials presented low intensity images. The order in which each sub-block was completed was counterbalanced across participants. Similarly, the images were randomly selected from the two stimulus subsets of high and low intensity (with the exclusion of images already presented in the Choice block) and counterbalanced for each participant.
Prior to commencing the actual task, participants completed eight practice trials including two instructed trials for each strategy at each intensity and four choice trials involving two trials at each intensity where participants chose between reappraisal and distraction. Each choice practice trial and each instructed practice trial were identical in chronological steps to the trials described in the Choice block and Instruct block, respectively. However, to ensure implementation of each strategy was correctly applied and understood, participants typed a brief description following the post-regulation rating phase in a comments box describing how they implemented each strategy. To evaluate participants’ adherence to correct implementation of each strategy, a judge blind to the strategy implemented for each trial coded which strategy the description matched. For the instructed reappraisal practice trials, 95.65% of participants’ descriptions were found to reference reappraisal, and for the distraction practice trials 85.87% were found to reference distraction. Across the four choice practice trials, adherence to strategy averaged to 93.34%, indicating a high level of understanding and correct implementation of each strategy as described.

Assessment of Experimental Task Adherence

To maintain the integrity of the collected data, participants were asked two questions on-screen at the end of the experimental session. For the first question, participants were first reminded that this was a genuine scientific study and the data collected intended for publication. They were then asked whether their responses could be used for this purpose. This included a brief description of reasons why their data should not be included, such as selecting random responses, closing their eyes, clicking through the task, or not trying to do the task properly. Participants responded with either selecting “my responses can be used or “my responses should not be used”. Next, participants were asked whether they frequently looked away from the images to deliberately avoid having to look at the negative content. Participants responded with either selecting “yes I frequently looked away” or “no I did not frequently look away”. To encourage honesty, participants were reassured for both questions that their responses would have no bearing on their course credit allocation.

Procedure

The current study was approved by the University of Western Australia’s Human Research Ethics Office (reference ET000074). Data was collected across two sessions. All participants provided informed consent prior to the commencement of each session. Participants completed both sessions privately in a small room with one computer.
In the first session, participants completed a demographics questionnaire, the NLESS-25, the DASS-21, the PANAS and the BRS, followed by delivery of the standardised adverse event. Participants then returned for a second experimental session within two weeks of attending the first session. In the second session, participants first viewed four example images obtained from the IAPS as an example of the type of content they would be exposed to. Informed consent was collected again before participants proceeded with the Emotion Regulation Assessment Task. Participants then completed the assessment of experimental task adherence, which marked the end of the experimental study. Participants were then thanked and debriefed about the purpose of the experiment.

Study Design, Transparency, and Openness

As the total testing duration of the experimental study would take approximately two hours to complete with a heavy burden on participants, and to avoid any carry-on effects of induced negative emotions from the standardised stressor task on responses to the Emotion Regulation Assessment Task and vice versa, it was decided it would be appropriate to divide the experiment across two sessions. In deciding which session was to be completed first, consideration was given towards an order that would minimise participant drop-out. As the Emotion Regulation Assessment Task has been associated with high drop-out rates in the past, we collected emotional resilience measures in the first session and measures of emotion regulation in the second. Note that the ordering of these two sequences does not carry implications around causality as this is functionally a cross-sectional design. Thus, given that the aim of the current study was to test for cross-sectional associations that were predicted by the proposed causal hypotheses, the division of testing sessions in this specific order was based on practical convenience. All analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (v29.0.2.0, 2023) except for the test of hypotheses which were analysed using R Statistical Software (v4.3.3; R Core Team, 2024). Specifically, all predictions were tested via Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modelling (PLS–SEM) in the SEMinR package (v2.3.4; Ray et al., 2024) as this method is less constrained to assumptions of causality and temporal ordering than other tests involving an explanatory variable (Sarstedt et al., 2020).
We report this study following JARS, including how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, all manipulations, and study measures (Cooper, 2008). The raw dataset has been made publicly available on the Open Science Framework. This study was not pre-registered.

Results

To test the predictions generated by each hypothesis required emerging adult participants with a quantifiable measure of emotional resilience. As the residual approach to measuring resilience should only be applied in people who have experienced adversity (Kalisch et al., 2021), only participants who scored 1 or more in their total NLESS score were included. Further, only those within the threshold for emerging adulthood (under 30; Arnett, 2007) were examined. Moreover, participants indicating their responses should not be used in the Assessment of Experimental Task Adherence (see Materials) were excluded from further analyses. Data of 149 participants, including 111 identifying as female, 37 as male and 1 as non-binary, with an age range of 17–26 (M = 20.02, SD = 1.95) were analysed to derive and compare each measure of emotional resilience. However, as the Emotion Regulation Assessment Task required participants to downregulate whilst viewing the presentation of each image, only participants who indicated that they did not frequently look away in the Assessment of Experimental Task Adherence were included in analyses pertaining to the hypotheses at test (n = 113). Descriptive statistics for all variables and measures used in the calculation of DASS Residual Index scores and the Lab Stressor Recovery Index scores are presented in Table 1.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for the variables and measures required for the derivation of the DASS Residual Index and Lab Stressor Recovery Index (N = 149)
Variable
M
SD
Min–Max
Skew
Kurtosis
Total NLESS score
3.80
2.43
1–12
1.21
1.56
Total DASS score
22.68
12.33
2–55
0.47
 − 0.54
State-Anxiety score
     
 Pre-adversity
12.02
3.56
6–21
0.29
 − 0.43
 Post-adversity
19.58
3.46
11–24
 − 0.41
 − 0.86
 Mid-recovery
14.49
3.75
7–24
0.18
 − 0.35
 End-recovery
12.38
3.69
6–24
0.33
 − 0.13

Residual-Approach for Calculation of the DASS Residual Index

The residual approach to calculating the DASS Residual Index rests on the assumption that number of past exposures to adversity is positively associated to level of current emotional difficulties (Kalisch et al., 2015; McLaughlin et al., 2010). As assumed, total NLESS scores and total DASS scores were significantly and positively correlated, r = .257, p = .002, permitting calculation of each participant’s DASS Residual Index score. Total DASS scores were first regressed on to total NLESS scores, yielding a significant model, F(1, 147) = 10.36, p = .002, R2 = .066, which confirmed experiencing past adversity significantly predicted an increase in emotional difficulties (B = 1.30, SE = 0.40, t = 3.22, p = .002, 95% CI = [0.50, 2.10]). The residual scores were then saved and reverse scored to represent each participant’s DASS Residual Index, where higher scores indicate fewer emotional difficulties than expected relative to the adversity experienced, thus reflects greater levels of the outcome-based measure of emotional resilience.

Calculation of the Lab Stressor Recovery Index

To permit calculation of each participant’s Lab Stressor Recovery Index, it was first confirmed that participants’ State-Anxiety scores significantly increased following exposure to the standardised stressor task (see Supplementary Materials for details of analysis and results). Participants’ Lab Stressor Recovery Index was then derived by calculating the amount of perseveration in state anxiety following exposure to the standardised stressor task. Given that reactivity and recovery has been shown as dissociable dimensions of a stress response and may have distinct underlying mechanisms (Degering et al., 2023; Linden et al., 1997), coupled with conceptualisations of emotional resilience that emphasise this construct as an outcome that can only be observed following stressor exposure (Bonanno & Diminich, 2013; Bonanno et al., 2015), only participants’ State-Anxiety scores measured during the recovery period were considered when deriving each participant’s Lab Stressor Recovery Index score. This index thus permits examination of mechanisms associated with individual differences in stress recovery as a separate, independent dimension from stress reactivity.
As per Rudaizky and MacLeod (2014), this was calculated as the mean in State-Anxiety scores measured at mid-recovery and end-recovery assessment points, where higher scores reflect greater preservation of state-anxiety following completion of the standardised stressor task. The mean State-Anxiety score measured across mid-recovery and end-recovery assessment points were subsequently inversed to derive participants’ Lab Stressor Recovery Index score. Higher Lab Stressor Recovery Index scores indicate less perseveration of state-anxiety following exposure to the standardised adversity delivered in the lab, thus reflects greater levels of a transient measure of emotional resilience operationalised as a dynamic construct.

Comparison of Measures in Assessing Distinct Operationalisations of Emotional Resilience

To evaluate the relationship between measures of emotional resilience reflecting distinct operationalisations, the correlation coefficients between measures were inspected. There is considerable consensus within the literature that a correlation at or above .80 between two distinct measures indicate they may be measuring synonymous constructs (Hodson, 2021). Pearson correlation analyses with 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals set at 1000 replications suggested that the DASS Residual Index appeared distinct from the Lab Stressor Recovery Index, r(147) = .34, p < .001, 95% BCI = [.19, .47], and distinct from the BRS Index, r(147) = .47, p < .001, 95% BCI = [.33, .59]. The Lab Stressor Recovery Index also appeared distinct from the BRS Index, r(147) = .24, p = .003, 95% BCI = [.08, .39]. These findings suggest each measure reflected distinct operationalisations of emotional resilience, and thus permit analyses and comparison of the proposed models on each measure separately entered as the dependent variable.

Assumption Underpinning the Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis

The Reappraisal Tendency Alignment hypothesis assumes that most participants would demonstrate the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon. Specifically, this hypothesis assumes that in the Choice block, participants would demonstrate a greater tendency to choose reappraisal for low intensity trials and distraction for high intensity trials.
To examine this assumption, the proportion of trials participants chose each strategy in the Choice block was subjected to a 2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA with within-subjects factor of Strategy (Reappraisal vs. Distraction) and Intensity (Low vs. High). As predicted and depicted in Fig. 2a, there was a significant Strategy × Intensity interaction, F(1,112) = 175.52, p < .001, ηp2 = .610. In line with previous research (Shafir et al., 2015; Sheppes et al., 2011; Van Bockstaele et al., 2020) follow-up paired-samples t-tests revealed participants chose reappraisal significantly more in low intensity trials (M = .66, SD = .25) than in high intensity trials (M = .29, SD = .20), t(112) = 13.24, p < .001, d = 1.25, and distraction significantly more in high intensity trials (M = .71, SD = .20) than in low intensity trials (M = .34, SD = .25), t(112) = 13.25, p < .001, d =  − 1.25. Thus, the assumption based on the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon underpinning the Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis was met.

Downregulation Effectiveness of Instructed Strategy Use

To explore participants negative emotion downregulation effectiveness when instructed to use each of the strategies to downregulate from high and low intensity images, participants’ instructed downregulation effectiveness scores derived from trials at each intensity level in the reappraisal and distraction sub-blocks of the Instruct block were examined. Instructed Downregulation Effectiveness scores were subjected to a 2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA, with within-subjects factor of Strategy (Reappraisal vs. Distraction) and Intensity (Low vs. High). As depicted in Fig. 2b, there was a significant interaction of Strategy × Intensity, F(1, 112) = 26.29, p < .001, ηp2 = .190. Follow-up paired samples t-test revealed that for high intensity trials, participants were significantly more effective at downregulating when instructed to use distraction (M = 1.75, SD = 1.33) than when instructed to use reappraisal (M = 1.28, SD = 1.14), t(112) = 4.97, p < .001, d = .47. For low intensity trials, there was no significant difference in the effectiveness of downregulation when instructed to use distraction (M = 1.31, SD = 0.93) compared to reappraisal (M = 1.24, SD = 1.00). Additionally, when examining the effectiveness of each strategy at each level of intensity, participants were significantly more effective at downregulating when instructed to use distraction in high intensity trials than in low intensity trials, t(112) = 4.86, p < .001, d = .46, but did not differ when instructed to use reappraisal in low intensity trials compared to high intensity trials, t =  − .46, p > .05.

Test of Hypotheses

Descriptive statistics and inter-correlations pertaining to all subsequent analyses are presented in Table 2. To test the predictions generated by both hypotheses under scrutiny required evaluation of direct and indirect associations between reappraisal choice tendencies, downregulation effectiveness and measures of emotional resilience reflecting distinct operationalisations. Therefore, a series of PLS–SEM analyses were tested. Additionally, exploratory analyses examining whether the predicted associations posited by both hypotheses would be conditional upon the extent to which individuals achieved effective downregulation when implementing reappraisal and/or distraction in general, or for specific stimuli was also conducted. Results of these exploratory analyses conducted on each emotional resilience measure are reported in Supplementary Materials. All models were bootstrapped with 1000 repetitions. Evidence of statistical significance for each pathway was indicated when bootstrapped confidence intervals did not pass through zero.
Table 2
Inter-correlations and descriptive statistics of all variables assessed in the testing of the Reappraisal Dominance Hypothesis and the Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis (n = 113)
 
1
2
3
4
5
6
Outcome variables
      
 DASS Residual Index
     
 Lab Stressor Recovery Index
.395**
    
 BRS Index
.455**
.327**
   
Predictor variables
      
 Reappraisal Tendency
.047
.206*
.208*
  
 Reappraisal Tendency Alignment
 − .005
.038
.052
.202*
  
 Downregulation Effectiveness
 − .085
 − .082
 − .049
 − .151
 − .051
M
0.65
 − 13.42
3.10
.47
0.69
1.49
SD
12.15
3.70
0.72
.17
0.15
0.98
**p < .001, *p < .01

The Reappraisal Dominance Hypothesis

To test the predictions generated by the Reappraisal Dominance Hypothesis, which posits direct and indirect associations between the tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction, effective downregulation, and emotional resilience, three separate PLS–SEM analyses were conducted. Reappraisal Tendency scores were entered as the independent variable, Downregulation Effectiveness scores as the intermediate variable, and the scores pertaining to each measure of emotional resilience (DASS Residual Index, Lab Stressor Recovery Index and BRS Index) were separately entered as the dependent variable. The direct and indirect pathways defined between each variable are depicted in Fig. 3.
First, the analyses revealed a non-significant direct pathway between Reappraisal Tendency scores and Downregulation Effectiveness scores, bootstrapped β =  − 0.15, SD = 0.11, t =  − 1.43, 95% CI [− 0.37, 0.04] (path a, Fig. 3). Secondly, the analyses revealed a non-significant direct pathway between Downregulation Effectiveness scores and the scores for each measure of emotional resilience (DASS Residual Index; bootstrapped β =  − 0.08, SD = 0.09, t =  − 0.89, 95% CI [− 0.24, 0.10], Lab Stressor Recovery Index; bootstrapped β =  − 0.05, SD = 0.12, t =  − 0.46, 95% CI [− 0.28, 0.17], BRS Index; bootstrapped β =  − 0.02, SD = 0.11, t =  − 0.17, 95% CI [− 0.23, 0.19] (path b, Fig. 3). Thirdly, the analyses revealed non-significant indirect pathways (path ab, Fig. 3) between Reappraisal Tendency scores, Downregulation Effectiveness scores and the scores for each measure of emotional resilience (DASS Residual Index; bootstrapped β = 0.01, SD = 0.02, t = 0.69, 95% CI [− 0.03, 0.04], Lab Stressor Recovery Index; bootstrapped β = 0.01, SD = 0.02, t = 0.37, 95% CI [− 0.04, 0.05], BRS Index; bootstrapped β = 0.00, SD = 0.02, t = 0.14, 95% CI [− 0.04, 0.04]. Thus, the predicted indirect associations between Reappraisal Tendency scores and scores for each measure of emotional resilience accounted for by Downregulation Effectiveness scores were not supported.
Lastly, the analyses confirmed significant direct pathways between Reappraisal Tendency scores and the Lab Stressor Recovery Index scores, bootstrapped β = 0.20, SD = 0.09, t = 2.13, 95% CI [0.02, 0.38], and between Reappraisal Tendency scores and the BRS Index scores, bootstrapped β = 0.02, SD = 0.10, t = 2.10, 95% CI [0.00, 0.39], but a non-significant direct pathway between Reappraisal Tendency scores and the DASS Residual Index scores, bootstrapped β = 0.04, SD = 0.11, t = 0.31, 95% CI [− 0.18, 0.25] (path c′, Fig. 3). Specifically, when controlling for Downregulation Effectiveness scores, greater Reappraisal Tendency scores were associated with greater Lab Stressor Recovery Index scores and with greater BRS Index scores. Thus, the predicted direct associations between Reappraisal Tendency scores and the scores for each measure of emotional resilience were partially supported.

The Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis

To test the predictions generated by the Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis, which posits direct and indirect associations between the tendency to choose reappraisal aligned to the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon (i.e., reappraisal tendency alignment), effective downregulation, and emotional resilience, three separate PLS–SEM analyses were conducted. Reappraisal Tendency Alignment scores were entered as the independent variable, Downregulation Effectiveness scores as the intermediate variable and scores pertaining to each measure of emotional resilience (DASS Residual Index, Lab Stressor Recovery Index and BRS Index) were separately entered as the dependent variable.
First, the analyses revealed non-significant direct pathways between Reappraisal Tendency Alignment scores and Downregulation Effectiveness scores, bootstrapped β =  − 0.05, SD = 0.11, t =  − 0.47, 95% CI [− 0.28, 0.15] (path a, Fig. 4). Secondly, the analyses revealed non-significant direct pathways between Downregulation Effectiveness scores and scores for each measure of emotional resilience (DASS Residual Index; bootstrapped β =  − 0.09, SD = 0.08, t =  − 1.03, 95% CI [− 0.24, 0.08], Lab Stressor Recovery Index; β =  − 0.08, SD = 0.11, t =  − 0.73, 95% CI [− 0.30, 0.13], BRS Index; β =  − 0.05, SD = 0.10, t =  − 0.45, 95% CI [− 0.26, 0.16] (path b, Fig. 4). Thirdly, the analyses revealed non-significant indirect pathways between Reappraisal Tendency Alignment scores and scores for each measure of emotional resilience via Downregulation Effectiveness scores (DASS Residual Index; bootstrapped β = 0.00, SD = 0.01, t = 0.32, 95% CI [− 0.02, 0.04], Lab Stressor Recovery Index; bootstrapped β = 0.00, SD = 0.02, t = 0.23, 95% CI [− 0.02, 0.05], BRS Index; bootstrapped β = 0.00, SD = 0.02, t = 0.16, 95% CI [− 0.02, 0.04]) (path ab, Fig. 4). Thus, the predicted indirect associations between Reappraisal Tendency Alignment scores and scores for each measure of emotional resilience accounted for by Downregulation Effectiveness scores were not supported.
Finally, the analyses further revealed non-significant direct pathways between Reappraisal Tendency Alignment scores and scores for each measure of emotional resilience (DASS Residual Index; bootstrapped β =  − 0.01, SD = 0.09, t =  − 0.12, 95% CI [− 0.18, 0.16], Lab Stressor Recovery Index; bootstrapped β = 0.03, SD = 0.10, t = 0.34, 95% CI [− 0.16, 0.22], BRS Index; bootstrapped β = 0.05, SD = 0.09, t = 0.53, 95% CI [− 0.13, 0.23]) (path c′, Fig. 4). Thus, the predicted direct association between Reappraisal Tendency Alignment scores and scores for each measure of emotional resilience were not supported.

Discussion

The current study examined the contribution of reappraisal choice tendencies as a potential cognitive mechanism to explain why some young adults show more emotional resilience than others. Due to a lack of clarity in the conceptualisation of emotional resilience resulting in large disparities in its operational definition and findings across studies (Bonanno et al., 2015; Hiebel et al., 2021; Kalisch et al., 2015; Luthar et al., 2000), cross-sectional associations generated by two candidate hypotheses were examined across three distinct operationalisations of emotional resilience: outcome-based, transient, and trait-based. Our results did not lend support for the predictions generated by the Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis, as reappraisal tendency aligned to the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon (i.e., tendency to choose reappraisal for low-intensity and distraction for high-intensity stimuli) was not associated with each measure of emotional resilience, nor was this relationship partially accounted for by downregulation effectiveness. However, partial support for the predictions generated by the Reappraisal Dominance Hypothesis was evident, as greater tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction was directly associated with greater levels of the transient measure of emotional resilience and with greater levels of the trait-based measure of perceived emotional resilience. Given that the present study is the first to examine these candidate hypotheses across three distinct operationalisations of emotional resilience, these findings provide important theoretical implications for our current understanding of the cognitive mechanisms underpinning individual differences in this construct. The implications of these findings will now be discussed, including limitations and considerations for future research.
Firstly, our results illuminate how differences in reappraisal choice tendencies may relate to variation in emotional resilience. Specifically, whilst our results are mostly consistent with the positive appraisal style theory of resilience (PASTOR) framework which emphasises reappraisal as a key mechanism for resilience (Kalisch et al., 2015; Riepenhausen et al., 2022), these results are contradictory to the suggestions proposed by the cognitive model of psychological resilience, which emphasises the role of cognitive flexibility based on contextual demands as a key mechanism underpinning emotionally resilient outcomes (Parsons et al., 2016). Indeed, there is a growing consensus within the literature which proposes that the congruency between emotion regulation strategies and contextual demands, rather than greater overall use of one specific strategy, is associated with greater emotional well-being and better mental health outcomes (Aldao et al., 2015; Bonanno & Burton, 2013). However, whilst our results replicate those of previous studies demonstrating the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon (Shafir et al., 2015, 2016; Sheppes et al., 2011), such that individuals indeed demonstrated a greater tendency to choose reappraisal for low intensity stimuli and distraction for high intensity stimuli, there was no evidence that a pattern of emotional regulation strategy choices that better aligned with this emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon was associated with emotional resilience.
A possible explanation for this might be illuminated by the regulatory selection options and the nature of the emotional stimuli adopted in the present study. Specifically, the current study was limited in examining only one form of lab-based manipulation involving two choices of emotion regulation strategies (i.e., reappraisal or distraction) to apply across an equal number of low intensity and high intensity images. Of course, individuals also deploy other forms of emotion regulation strategies in response to other forms of contextual demands (Kobylińska & Kusev, 2019). Thus, these findings do not exclude flexibility across other types of emotion regulation strategies as a factor related to variation in emotional resilience. Specifically, given our significant findings concerning the relationship between heightened tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction and emotional resilience, it is possible that flexibility between reappraisal and another strategy commonly deployed to other forms of contextual demands is related to emotional resilience.
For example, the emotion regulation strategy of acceptance, which involves engaging with, and non-judgementally accepting, one’s own negative emotions as opposed to attempts to change it (Wojnarowska et al., 2020), has been shown to be positively related to self-reported trait resilience (Min et al., 2013; Polizzi et al., 2018). Furthermore, a study by Troy et al. (2018) found that acceptance was more effective than reappraisal towards reducing physiological arousal, whilst reappraisal was more effective than acceptance in reducing subjective distress. Thus, future studies may benefit from examining flexibility in emotion regulation strategy choice between reappraisal and acceptance by manipulating negative emotion eliciting events causing heightened subjective distress relative to heightened physiological arousal, as a potential mechanism underpinning emotion resilience.
Secondly, our results advance current theoretical understanding of emotional resilience as a construct. Consider our finding that greater tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction was differentially associated with each measure of emotional resilience. While our results are consistent with previous research concerning the relationship between self-reported reappraisal use and trait-based measures of perceived emotional resilience (Mestre et al., 2017), they are only partially consistent with the propositions made by the PASTOR framework regarding the relationship between reappraisal and emotionally resilient outcomes (Kalisch et al., 2015). Specifically, when emotional resilience was operationalised to reflect conceptualisations which view this construct as an outcome of a dynamic process, as per the transient and outcome-based measures of emotional resilience, tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction was only associated with the former. Interestingly, our exploratory analysis (see Supplementary Materials) revealed a conditional direct association between reappraisal tendency and the outcome-based measure when a moderator was added to the model. Specifically, a greater tendency to choose reappraisal was associated with higher levels of the outcome-based measure but only for those individuals who were less effective at downregulating when instructed to implement reappraisal relative to when instructed to implement distraction. Conversely, for those who were more effective at downregulating with reappraisal than distraction, this relationship was reversed. One plausible explanation for the observed differential pattern of findings is that different mechanisms may relate to different dimensions of the same construct.
This interpretation is consistent with theories viewing emotional resilience as a multi-dimensional and dynamic construct, as opposed to being unidimensional and static (Miller-Graff, 2022; Rutter, 2012). The significant, positive, and moderately sized correlations evident between each measure of emotional resilience reflecting distinct operationalisations is consistent with this notion. From a statistical standpoint, measures of distinct theoretical constructs should not correlate perfectly, or near perfectly, with another construct (Hodson, 2021). Thus, these moderate-sized correlations indicate that while the three measures of emotional resilience appear to be related, which can be argued to be theoretically expected, our results support distinguishing the different measures as distinct dimensions of emotional resilience.
Theories suggesting that individuals indeed may show strong emotional resilience in one dimension but may not necessarily translate into showing strong emotional resilience in another further supports this notion (Luthar, 2015; Luthar et al., 2000; Rutter, 2012). Thus, future researchers aiming to examine individual differences of this construct should engage with and compare the distinct operationalisations. The adoption of such methods can serve to improve the specificity of empirical findings, establish the extent of similarities and divergences between operationalisations, and allow for direct comparisons within specific contexts and across time. Establishing the mechanisms underpinning each operationalisation of emotional resilience reflecting distinct conceptualisations can allow future researchers aiming to promote emotional resilience in young adults to develop more effective and tailored interventions based on the specific dimension of interest (Hamby et al., 2018; Miller-Graff, 2022).
Finally, our results elucidate current understanding concerning the mechanisms that may explain the relationship between reappraisal choice tendencies and emotional resilience. Firstly, contrary to previous research indicating reappraisal and distraction as effective emotion regulation strategies (Webb et al., 2012), our results provided no evidence that tendency to choose reappraisal in general, or reappraisal aligned to the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon, was associated with more effective downregulation. Secondly, our finding that the degree of effectiveness of downregulation attempts was not associated with each measure of emotional resilience is inconsistent with previous evidence suggesting positive associations between self-reported effective emotion regulation and emotional resilience (Polizzi et al., 2021). These findings imply that downregulation effectiveness as measured in the current study cannot account for the proposed relationships between reappraisal choice tendencies and emotional resilience.
As per Fiedler et al. (2018), it is critical that researchers consider alternative explanatory variables and causal models when testing for associations between multiple variables as there are many possibilities that could explain significant and non-significant findings. Thus, it should be noted that while the current study examined the predictions of hypotheses reflecting causal models in an order that was consistent with previous evidence, downregulation effectiveness is only one candidate among many possible explanatory variables. It is possible that, for the significant direct associations evident with the transient and trait-based measures of emotional resilience specifically, alternative explanatory variables may be influencing this relationship. Previous studies that have examined the positive association between self-reported reappraisal use and trait-based measures of perceived emotional resilience in a sample of university students, suggest greater self-reported mindfulness (Zarotti et al., 2020) and greater self-reported self-esteem (Mouatsou & Koutra, 2023) as mechanisms that may account for variation in this relationship. Additionally, it is possible that the direction of the proposed relationships is bi-directional or reverse. For example, Hoorelbeke et al.’s (2016) network analysis found bi-directional associations between self-reported adaptive emotion regulation, which includes reappraisal use, and resilience, and Ford et al. (2017)’s findings suggest the possibility of a reverse relationship between reappraisal choice tendency and reappraisal implementation effectiveness. Thus, it is imperative that future work also consider alternative temporal ordering in causal models seeking to further investigate the relationship between emotion regulation and emotional resilience.

Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research

Despite its strengths, our study is not without limitations. Firstly, the current study was cross-sectional by design, as no variables were directly manipulated nor repeatedly measured. Thus, the results of the PLS–SEM analyses provide correlational evidence only and no inferences about the causal role and causal ordering of reappraisal choice tendencies as a candidate mechanism underpinning emotional resilience can be made. Future research seeking to elucidate causality and directionality between reappraisal choice tendencies and emotional resilience via downregulation effectiveness should adopt longitudinal cross-lagged panel designs involving repeated measures across sessions. Alternatively, future studies may seek to directly manipulate reappraisal choice tendencies and examine whether such manipulation directly influences differences in emotional resilience.
Secondly, the current sample consisted of young undergraduate students recruited from a Western, educated, industrialised, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) society (Henrich et al., 2010). Whilst our aim was to examine emotional resilience shown by young adults, the findings of this study may not be broadly applicable due to the homogeneity of the sample, which limits generalizability across diverse cultural contexts. Moreover, given that emotional resilience has shown to exhibit sensitivity to cultural and contextual influences (Ungar, 2008), future studies may wish to focus on addressing these limitations by adopting culturally sensitive methodologies and exploring intercultural differences in emotional resilience. For example, cultural-specific definitions and demonstrations of what constitutes as healthy emotional functioning following adversity, and assessments of culturally relevant stressors specific to the study’s target population should be considered (Blessin et al., 2022). Addressing these limitations and advancing emotional resilience research in this direction will aid in developing effective and culturally sensitive interventions aimed to promote emotional resilience across diverse populations.
Finally, while the study achieved strong experimental control, the study design was limited in achieving a high degree of ecological validity. As previously stated, the Emotion Regulation Assessment Task constraints the participants to select a strategy from two regulatory options to downregulate whilst viewing negative images. It is possible that the way individuals responded in this task is not a true reflection of how they may respond in real life. Future studies aiming to enhance ecological validity may benefit from examining emotion regulation choice tendencies in more naturalistic settings such as through ecological momentary assessments (see Boemo et al., 2022).

Conclusion

The current study adds to the existing literature by examining the contribution of emotion regulation, specifically reappraisal choice tendencies, as a potential mechanism underpinning individual differences across three distinct operationalisations of emotional resilience. In line with the PASTOR framework (Kalisch et al., 2015), our findings indicate that greater tendency to choose reappraisal over distraction was associated with greater emotional resilience operationalised as a transient state and a perceived trait. Additionally, our findings support theoretical models of emotional resilience as a multi-dimensional construct, suggesting that different mechanisms may relate to distinct operationalisations of emotional resilience. Critically, the cross-sectional design of the current study limits inferences concerning causality and directionality, thus future research aiming to replicate and extend on these findings are warranted. Nevertheless, our findings highlight the role of reappraisal use as a potential candidate mechanism underpinning emotional resilience and the need for future research investigating individual differences to engage with and compare the distinct operationalisations of emotional resilience. By doing so, a more comprehensive understanding of this construct can be achieved which can ultimately lead to the development of more targeted interventions for promoting emotional resilience in young adults.

Declarations

Conflict of Interest

Hannah A. Razak, Colin MacLeod, Daniel Rudaizky, and Lies Notebaert declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

BSL GOP_opleiding GZ-psycholoog

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Voetnoten
1
We considered the Reappraisal Dominance Hypothesis may only be valid for individuals for whom implementing reappraisal indeed leads to more effective downregulation than implementing distraction. Similarly, the Reappraisal Tendency Alignment Hypothesis may only be valid for individuals for whom the emotion regulation asymmetry phenomenon was indeed an effective pattern of emotion regulation strategy choice for negative emotion downregulation. Thus, exploratory analyses involving an additional moderating factor on each of the proposed models were conducted and can be found in the Supplementary Materials.
 
2
The current study utilised an emotional difficulties assessment to index the outcome-based measure of emotional resilience as difficulties in emotion regulation has been shown to increase risk for heightened distress and various psychopathologies (Aldao et al., 2015; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). However, theorists have argued that good emotional well-being following adversity also encompasses positive emotional states (Davydov et al., 2010), which are distinct and independent from negative emotional states (Watson & Naragon, 2012). Therefore, an outcome-based operationalisation of emotional resilience measure indexed with an emotional wellbeing assessment including positive and negative items was also included. Details of this measure and subsequent analyses can be found in the online Supplementary Materials.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Buri, J. R., Post, M. C., Cromett, C. E., Landis, A. M., & Alliegro, M. C. (2015). Negative life events scale for students (NLESS). College Student Journal, 52(3), 361–367. Buri, J. R., Post, M. C., Cromett, C. E., Landis, A. M., & Alliegro, M. C. (2015). Negative life events scale for students (NLESS). College Student Journal, 52(3), 361–367.
go back to reference IBM Corp. (2023). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 29.0.2.0). IBM Corp. IBM Corp. (2023). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 29.0.2.0). IBM Corp.
go back to reference Kalisch, R., Baker, D. G., Basten, U., Boks, M. P., Bonanno, G. A., Brummelman, E., Chmitorz, A., Fernàndez, G., Fiebach, C. J., Galatzer-Levy, I., Geuze, E., Groppa, S., Helmreich, I., Hendler, T., Hermans, E. J., Jovanovic, T., Kubiak, T., Lieb, K., Lutz, B., …, Kleim, B. (2017). The resilience framework as a strategy to combat stress-related disorders. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(11), 784–790. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0200-8 Kalisch, R., Baker, D. G., Basten, U., Boks, M. P., Bonanno, G. A., Brummelman, E., Chmitorz, A., Fernàndez, G., Fiebach, C. J., Galatzer-Levy, I., Geuze, E., Groppa, S., Helmreich, I., Hendler, T., Hermans, E. J., Jovanovic, T., Kubiak, T., Lieb, K., Lutz, B., …, Kleim, B. (2017). The resilience framework as a strategy to combat stress-related disorders. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(11), 784–790. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1038/​s41562-017-0200-8
go back to reference Kalisch, R., Köber, G., Binder, H., Ahrens, K. F., Basten, U., Chmitorz, A., Choi, K. W., Fiebach, C. J., Goldbach, N., Neumann, R. J., Kampa, M., Kollmann, B., Lieb, K., Plichta, M. M., Reif, A., Schick, A., Sebastian, A., Walter, H., Wessa, M., …, Engen, H. (2021). The frequent stressor and mental health monitoring-paradigm: A proposal for the operationalization and measurement of resilience and the identification of resilience processes in longitudinal observational studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.710493 Kalisch, R., Köber, G., Binder, H., Ahrens, K. F., Basten, U., Chmitorz, A., Choi, K. W., Fiebach, C. J., Goldbach, N., Neumann, R. J., Kampa, M., Kollmann, B., Lieb, K., Plichta, M. M., Reif, A., Schick, A., Sebastian, A., Walter, H., Wessa, M., …, Engen, H. (2021). The frequent stressor and mental health monitoring-paradigm: A proposal for the operationalization and measurement of resilience and the identification of resilience processes in longitudinal observational studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2021.​710493
go back to reference Kieling, C., Buchweitz, C., Caye, A., Silvani, J., Ameis, S. H., Brunoni, A. R., Cost, K. T., Courtney, D. B., Georgiades, K., Merikangas, K. R., Henderson, J. L., Polanczyk, G. V., Rohde, L. A., Salum, G. A., & Szatmari, P. (2024). Worldwide prevalence and disability from mental disorders across childhood and adolescence: Evidence from the global burden of disease study. JAMA Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1001/JAMAPSYCHIATRY.2023.5051CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kieling, C., Buchweitz, C., Caye, A., Silvani, J., Ameis, S. H., Brunoni, A. R., Cost, K. T., Courtney, D. B., Georgiades, K., Merikangas, K. R., Henderson, J. L., Polanczyk, G. V., Rohde, L. A., Salum, G. A., & Szatmari, P. (2024). Worldwide prevalence and disability from mental disorders across childhood and adolescence: Evidence from the global burden of disease study. JAMA Psychiatry. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1001/​JAMAPSYCHIATRY.​2023.​5051CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion-regulation choice. Source. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1391–1396.CrossRefPubMed Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion-regulation choice. Source. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1391–1396.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wang, K., Goldenberg, A., Dorison, C. A., Miller, J. K., Uusberg, A., Lerner, J. S., Gross, J. J., Agesin, B. B., Bernardo, M., Campos, O., Eudave, L., Grzech, K., Ozery, D. H., Jackson, E. A., Garcia, E. O. L., Drexler, S. M., Jurković, A. P., Rana, K., Wilson, J. P., …, Moshontz, H. (2021). A multi-country test of brief reappraisal interventions on emotions during the COVID-19 pandemic. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(8), 1089–1110. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01173-x Wang, K., Goldenberg, A., Dorison, C. A., Miller, J. K., Uusberg, A., Lerner, J. S., Gross, J. J., Agesin, B. B., Bernardo, M., Campos, O., Eudave, L., Grzech, K., Ozery, D. H., Jackson, E. A., Garcia, E. O. L., Drexler, S. M., Jurković, A. P., Rana, K., Wilson, J. P., …, Moshontz, H. (2021). A multi-country test of brief reappraisal interventions on emotions during the COVID-19 pandemic. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(8), 1089–1110. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1038/​s41562-021-01173-x
Metagegevens
Titel
The Role of Emotion Regulation in Distinct Measures of Emotional Resilience
Auteurs
Hannah A. Razak
Colin MacLeod
Daniel Rudaizky
Lies Notebaert
Publicatiedatum
18-02-2025
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Cognitive Therapy and Research
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-025-10581-6