Skip to main content

Welkom bij Scalda & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Scalda heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen.Je kunt de producten hieronder links aanschaffen en rechts inloggen.

Registreer

Schaf de BSL Academy aan: 

BSL Academy mbo AG

Eenmaal aangeschaft kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met Jan van der Velden.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top

Open Access 25-01-2025 | Original Article

The Role of Information Processing as an Underlying Mechanism in the Experience of Anxiety Reactivity and Anxiety Perseveration, Two Dissociable Dimensions of Trait Anxiety

Auteurs: Sophia Moore, Laura Dondzilo, Mahdi Mazidi, Colin MacLeod, Lies Notebaert

Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Purpose

While long considered a unitary dimension, research has moved towards a multidimensional understanding of trait anxiety, and has identified two dissociable dimensions of trait anxiety: anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration. Despite the consolidation of this understanding over the past decade, the mechanisms underlying these two dimensions are not well established. The present study examined the novel role of information processing in the experience of anxiety reactivity and perseveration, specifically, how enhanced processing of situational information influences reactivity, and how enhanced processing of implicational information influences perseveration.

Method

Undergraduate university students (N = 142) were exposed to an interview-based stressor. As participants approached this stressor, they were provided with information reflecting one of the two content categories, intended to encourage a situational or implicational processing mode. Anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration were measured by assessing changes in state anxiety (measured using a single item visual analogue state anxiety measure) in the lead up and wake of this stressor.

Results

Mixed-design ANOVAs did not reveal a significant relationship between enhanced processing of situational information and anxiety reactivity, nor between enhanced processing of implicational information and anxiety perseveration. An experimental manipulation involving valence revealed that receiving positive information reduced the intensity of state anxiety elevations in the aftermath of the information processing task.

Conclusions

Findings are discussed with reference to limitations and future extensions that could investigate the potential role of cognitive biases in this proposed relationship between enhanced information processing and anxiety reactivity and perseveration.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10608-025-10576-3.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Anxiety is the most prevalent mental health concern worldwide, with approximately 301 million people experiencing an anxiety related condition in 2019 alone (World Health Organisation, 2023). Not all experiences of anxiety are maladaptive, often serving the adaptive purpose of prompting individuals to avoid potential environmental threats (Gutiérrez-García & Contreras, 2013). However, when the intensity and frequency of anxiety symptoms is out of proportion to the current situation, this can cause significant emotional and cognitive disturbances, limiting the quality of life for the affected individual (Gutiérrez-García & Contreras, 2013). Due to the significant individual variability in the experience of anxiety, research has focused on identifying the mechanisms underpinning the frequency with which people experience anxiety symptoms, with the goal of limiting the impairment associated with this prevalent mental health concern (Ouimet at el., 2009). A mechanism of particular interest has been the manner in which people process information, with people often choosing to process information in a way that fits with their worldview and perceptions of self (Ouimet et al., 2009). Prominent cognitive theories of anxiety describe how people with anxiety often seek out and select information which matches their perceptions of the world as threatening or dangerous (Beck & Clark, 1997). Inherent in this process, is a matched perception that they do not have adequate resources to cope with this threat, prompting actions such as impaired disengagement with the threat or avoidance (Ouimet et al., 2009). As a result, individuals with anxiety-related dysfunction or anxiety disorders, experience hypervigilance for cues that indicate a potential threat and are at an increased likelihood of misinterpreting benign cues as dangerous, prompting unnecessary evasive actions, limiting their engagement with the outside world (Beck & Clark, 1997).
The frequency with which people experience anxiety symptoms is termed trait anxiety and is a well-established predictor of anxiety-related dysfunction (Endler & Kocovski, 2001). Anxiety is distinct from fear, in that anxiety is a more diffuse, future-oriented emotional state focusing on potential or anticipated threats (Grogans et al., 2023), whereas fear is tied to an immediate stimulus and therefore present-focused (Beck & Emery, 2005). In addition, anxiety involves more elaborate cognitive elaboration and anticipatory thinking (Grogans et al., 2023). Trait anxiety has been identified as a factor contributing to the aetiology of anxiety-related disorders (Knowles & Olatunji, 2020), making research into the construct itself, and the contributing mechanisms, an important element in extending our understanding of anxiety.
Until recently, trait anxiety has commonly been considered a unitary dimension that is frequently assessed using questionnaires such as the trait version of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T; Spielberger et al., 1983), which requires individuals to report on the frequency with which they generally experience specific anxiety symptoms (Julian, 2011) (see method section and Knowles & Olatunji (2020) for review of STAI-T discriminant validity concerns with depressive symptomatology). Theoretically, however, a number of different types of anxious disposition could underlie variation in trait anxiety scores. Research by Rudaizky and colleagues (2012, 2014) has revealed that two dimensions of anxious disposition each account for unique variance in trait anxiety scores (Rudaizky et al., 2012). One dimension, referred to as anxiety reactivity, reflects individual differences in the magnitude of state anxiety responses elicited by a stressor. For example, in response to an impending examination, most people would experience some elevation in state anxiety. Individuals who experience greater reactivity would exhibit more frequent and intense state anxiety reactions. The other dimension of trait anxiety, referred to as anxiety perseveration, reflects individual differences in the persistence of a state anxiety elevation, once induced by a stressor (Rudaizky et al., 2012). For example, following an examination, most people would experience anxiety reducing emotions such as relief. Individuals who experience greater perseveration would remain anxious for longer after leaving the examination room.
Over the last decade, the suggestion has been made within the literature that different cognitive mechanisms may independently drive variation in anxiety reactivity and in anxiety perseveration (Gole et al., 2012). However, to the authors’ knowledge, the present study is the first to systematically assess candidate cognitive mechanisms, specifically information processing, underlying these two dimensions of anxiety vulnerability. Cognitive theorists have suggested that individual differences in anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration reflect the differing impact of alternative types of information which people process (Rudaizky et al., 2014). Building on this foundation, the present study’s hypotheses were derived from literature focusing on similar emotional processes with well-established cognitive underpinnings (Kross et al., 2005; Watkins et al., 2008). Relevant to understanding individual differences in anxiety reactivity is literature focusing on emotional regulation (Davis et al., 2011). Research by Kross and colleagues (2005, 2014) posits that thinking about a situation in a manner that involves focusing on situational details, leads to heightened emotional arousal. More specifically, by using a self-immersed perspective where one prioritises processing and visualising the emotionally arousing and specific elements of a situation (e.g., the feelings elicited and challenging aspects), individuals run the risk of experiencing enhanced physiological stress and greater emotional reactivity (Kross & Ayduk, 2017). This model suggests that individuals who are prone to taking a self-immersed perspective, focusing on tangible and emotional aspects of a situation (i.e., situational information), are more likely to experience increased emotional reactivity (Kross & Ayduk, 2017), a process not dissimilar to anxiety reactivity. There is empirical evidence to support this account. For example, a study by Bornstein and colleagues (2020) identified that when participants tended to process situational details concerning a stressful event (e.g., writing how and in what order the event happened), they experienced greater elevation of negative emotions than did those who tended to process more self-distanced and abstract details. Though Kross and colleagues did not examine anxiety reactivity specifically, their findings suggest that elevated anxiety reactivity could be driven by the processing of situational information concerning the stressor. Therefore, the first hypothesis under test in the current study is that processing situational information about a stressful event leads to greater anxiety reactivity (but not to greater perseveration) in response to this event.
Relevant to understanding anxiety perseveration is literature focusing on repetitive thoughts, such as depressive rumination (a process akin to anxiety perseveration). Within this literature base, there is evidence to suggest that preferential processing of information, which is decontextualised and abstract in nature, is one of the crucial processes which differentiates dysfunctional from functional repetitive thoughts (Watkins & Teasdale, 2004). More specifically, Watkins’ (2008) Processing Mode Theory posits that utilising an implicational processing mode inhibits adaptive emotional processing of negative/stressful events (White & Wild, 2016). By disrupting the formation of a cohesive and specific narrative concerning a situation or event, negative interpretations are strengthened, giving rise to repetitive negative, or intrusive thoughts (White & Wild, 2016). Thus, Watkins and colleague propose that processing implication information which focuses on the reasons for, and the consequences of an event, increases repetitive negative thinking and so elevates negative affect (Watkins et al., 2008).
There is evidence to support Watkin’s theory, with increased processing of implicational information processing found to elevate negative affect (Kambara et al., 2019). Most relevant to the present research, one study identified that individuals who were presented with statements that contained abstract thoughts about the reasons for and the consequences of a past event experienced longer duration of negative mood and arousal following a stressful event (Ehring et al., 2009). Another study by Altan-Atalay and colleagues (2022) exposed participants to a stressful situation and employed a manipulation that required participants to either process implication information (by answering questions such as “why is this happening to me?”), or non-implicational information (by answering questions such as “how can I solve the problem at hand?”). They found that participants who were induced to process implicational information experienced higher levels of subsequent state rumination (Altan-Atalay et al., 2022). While these studies provide support for Watkin’s theory, their methodology does not distinguish between the impact that processing implicational information has on anxiety perseveration. Therefore, the current literature base is unable to determine whether the processing of implicational information contributes to elevated anxiety perseveration specifically. However, given Watkins’ theory and this preliminary empirical support, it is plausible to suggest that elevated anxiety perseveration could be increased by the processing of implicational information. The second hypothesis tested in the current study is therefore that processing information which is implicational in nature leads to greater anxiety perseveration (but not anxiety reactivity) in the wake of a stressful event.
Distinguishing the cognitive mechanisms that differentially contribute to variation in anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration, respectively, has important theoretical implications for advancing the understanding of individual differences in trait anxiety. Cognitive models of anxiety, while recognising that focusing on negative content elevates trait anxiety, do not differentiate between the distinctive ways in which information can be processed (i.e. the nature of this negative focus) that may differentially give rise to elevated anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration (Rudaizky et al., 2012). By exploring the influence of such information processing styles, it becomes possible for us to better understand how varied information and/or content can influence individuals’ responses to stressful events, in both the short and longer term. This opens the possibility of examining how manipulating the information people process prior to a stressful event, impacts the level of anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration they experience both throughout and after the event (Rudaizky et al., 2012).
The current study sought to test these two hypotheses by examining whether participants who process either situational or implication information about an upcoming stressor respectively exhibit heighted anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration in response to this event. Participants were told that would be exposed to a lab-based stressor, which took the form of a simulated interview. At several junctures across the experimental session, both as they approached this stressor and also after they had completed it, participants’ state anxiety was assessed. This made it possible to compute separate measures of their anxiety reactivity (i.e., reflecting state anxiety elevation elicited by the approaching interview) and anxiety perseveration (i.e., reflecting persistence of anxiety following the interview). To examine the impact of processing either situational or implicational information on each dimension of anxiety response, every participant was required to process either one or the other type of information concerning the stressor. This information was presented in the form of video clips, in which supposed former participants shared either situational information about the interview (i.e. what they experienced in the moment) or implicational information about the interview (i.e. what the consequences of the interview were for them).
While information type was the main experimental manipulation, consideration was given to the valence of the information, given the wealth of research that implicates negative information in the experience of anxiety (Mogg & Bradley, 2016) and rumination (Watkins & Roberts, 2020). For example, there is extensive evidence which implicates biased attentional processing of negative information in the experience of trait anxiety (Bar-Haim et al., 2007). Negative information, in this context, is threat-related and therefore focuses on threatening aspects of the Interview Challenge. Conversely, positive information is threat-incongruent, focusing on neutral or pleasant aspects of the Interview Challenge. This gave rise to an exploratory manipulation whereby each information type category was broken into two separate conditions: positive and negative. Given the well-established link between negatively valenced information and elevated anxiety-linked responses (Ouimet et al., 2009; Mogg & Bradley, 2016), it is plausible to suggest that only the processing of negative situational and implicational information would give rise to anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration, respectively. To ensure that any effects observed could be attributed to the processing of negative situational or implicational information (and not to the processing of situational or implicational information more generally) this between-subjects manipulation led to participants being presented either with exclusively negative or exclusive positive (implicational or situational) information about the upcoming interview.
The hypotheses predict that participants who are required to process negative information concerning the upcoming Interview Challenge Stressor (see experimental tasks p. 14) that is situational in nature, as compared to those who are required to process negative information that is implicational in nature or that is positive, will experience greater anxiety reactivity (but not perseveration) during the period when they are introduced to the Interview Challenge. Additionally, it is predicted that participants who are required to process negative information concerning the upcoming Interview Challenge that is implicational in nature, as compared to those who are required to process negative information that is situational in nature or that is positive, will experience greater anxiety perseveration (but not reactivity) in the period following participation in the Interview Challenge.

Method

Participants

One hundred and forty-one undergraduate psychology students were recruited via a university online research participation system. Using G*Power version 3.1.9.7 (Faul et al., 2007), an a priori power analysis was conducted to determine the minimum sample size required to test the experimental hypotheses. To achieve sufficient power to detect a medium effect size (f = 0.25) with 80% probability, with a conservative 0.20 non-sphericity correction applied, a sample size of 116 was recommended. Therefore, it can be assumed the obtained sample of 141 participants is sufficiently large to test the current hypotheses. Across the 141 participants (111 females, 29 males, and one non-binary/other), ages ranged from 17 to 59 (M = 19.70, SD = 5.46). Eighty-three participants (59%) identified their ethnicity as Caucasian, 52 participants (37%) reported they are of Asian descent, four participants (3%) identified as Hispanic, and two participants (1%) reported they are of Middle Eastern descent. Participants were compensated with partial course credit for taking part in the current study, which was approved by the University of Western Australia’s Human Research Ethics Office (2021/ET000074). This research was conducted in accordance with the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Human Research (2023) and all research procedures were completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. Participants gave written informed consent and were able to terminate the experiment at any time.

Materials

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al., 1983) was administered to measure individual differences in participants’ trait anxiety at the commencement of the experimental session. The 20-item trait anxiety scale (STAI-T) asks individuals to report on the frequency of their general feelings of anxiety by indicating how they “generally feel”, for example, “I am happy”. Responses are provided using a 4-point Likert scale which ranges from ‘Almost Never’, to ‘Almost Always’, with higher scores indicating greater trait anxiety. The STAI-T has well established internal consistently with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from good (0.86) to excellent (0.95) across varied population groups, including undergraduate students (Notebaert et al., 2020). Additionally, the STAI-T demonstrates adequate (0.73) to good (0.85) convergent validity with other widely used measures of anxiety (e.g., the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale and the Anxiety Scale Questionnaire) (Maynard et al., 2010). In the current sample, the STAI-T demonstrated excellent internal consistency (a = 0.93). It is important to note that some contemporary articles have raised concerns that the STAI-T lacks discriminant validity, based on relationships observed between the STAI-T and measures of depression (Balsamo et al., 2013; Knowles & Olatunji, 2020). In line with this, researchers have queried whether some of the items (e.g., the reverse scored “I am happy”) in fact measure depressive symptomatology, rather than a trait disposition to experience anxiety (Knowles & Olatunji, 2020).

Visual Analogue State Anxiety Measure

In order to permit computation of variation in anxiety reactivity and in anxiety perseveration, state anxiety was assessed at critical junctures across the experimental session, using a one-item visual analogue anxiety measure, adapted from a measure developed by Davey et al. (2007). A number of these measurements were taken in the lead up to the experimental stressor, to assess the intensity of state anxiety elevations (forming the basis for the anxiety reactivity assessment). Additionally, multiple measurements were taken following the experimental stressor to measure the persistence of state anxiety elevations (forming the basis for the anxiety perseveration assessment). Additional information regarding the temporal location of these measurement points can be found in the Procedure section and Fig. 3. On each such assessment, participants were presented with a 10 cm horizontal line on the screen, anchored at the left end with the words “not at all anxious” and anchored at the right with the words “extremely anxious”. Instructions were presented on the screen which asked participants to “Use the mouse to move the marker on the screen to the point on the scale that accurately reflects how anxious you currently are”. Davey et al. (2007) demonstrated that the visual analogue version of the single item state anxiety scale has adequate predictive validity (p =.78) with the STAI-S (STAI; Spielberger et al., 1983), a highly reliable (Cronbach’s a range 0.86 to 0.95; Spielberger et al., 1983) measure of state anxiety. Given the recurrent nature of anxiety assessments throughout the experimental session, a one item measure of state anxiety was chosen due to its brief duration and sensitivity in detecting subtle differences in participants emotional experience across time.

Stimuli

Video Clip Stimuli

To enable the processing of information that is situational or implicational in nature, and negative or positive in valence, 16 video clips were created. These video clips were split evenly across four content categories: negative situational, positive situational, negative implicational, and positive implicational content. These video clips presented “actors”, students between the ages of 18 and 25, recruited from the community, who delivered pre-written scripts. These scripts were developed by the research team to present situational or implicational information, that was also positive or negative in nature. Situational information included reference to specific and concrete aspects of the Interview Challenge and how these influenced mood (e.g., a participant feeling judged by the interviewer in the moment). Implicational information included reference to consequences and implications stemming from participation in the Interview Challenge (e.g., struggling to regain confidence after a poor performance). Negative information is threat-relevant, focusing on threatening aspects of the Interview Challenge and associated emotions (e.g., sadness, fear, and anger), while positive information relates to neutral or pleasant aspects of the Interview Challenge and associated emotions (e.g., happiness, contentment, and relaxed). Each video had a duration of precisely one minute.
Statements used to build the video scripts were generated using a systematic process. The lead author generated statements to commence each script, which clearly identified the valence and information type. From this starting point, additional statements were generated, which presented information from the category the video would represent. Once each script contained sufficient statements, they were validated by obtaining perfect inter-rater reliability for each statement, or revision of identified statements would occur until there was unanimous agreement obtained among three experts in the field, that these statements mapped on to the intended category (see online supplementary materials for further details concerning video development and example scripts).
Using Adobe Premier Pro® (Version 22.0, Adobe Inc., 2022), the sixteen video clips were edited to ensure uniform size, clarity, and duration. Each video measured 26 cm x 26 cm and was positioned in the centre of the screen.

Experimental Tasks

Information Processing Task

The purpose of this task was to require participants to process one of the four categories of information (situational negative information, situational positive information, implicational negative information, or implicational positive information) concerning the forthcoming event, to determine how processing information differing in valence and content influences anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration. In each condition, participants were exposed to four video clips, which presented information from the same information valence and information type categories (e.g., situational positive), with a short break between the presentation of each clip. Each participant was exposed to one category of information only. Participants were iteratively assigned to one of the four conditions using a unique identifier, to ensure an approximately even distribution of the sample across conditions (Situational Negative n = 34, Situational Positive n = 34, Implicational Negative n = 35, Implicational Positive n = 35). The order the four video clips played in remained constant across participants, for each respective condition. The clips were presented in the centre of the screen 6 cm from the left and right edges of the screen (see Fig. 1 for visual exemplar of stimuli presentation). Participants were told to “please pay close attention, as you will be asked questions about these video clips later.” This instruction was included to ensure that participants adequately attended to the video clip stimuli. Audio was played out loud, via the computer speaker, at a consistent volume for each participant.

Interview Challenge Stressor

The Interview Challenge stressor was modelled on the interview platform Vieple (Vieple, 2022), which is a video interviewing platform commonly utilised for remote and first round interviews. The platform allows for standardised and structured interviews to be conducted, with candidates answering pre-prepared questions via video recordings. The Interview Challenge stressor followed this general set up by presenting participants with questions on a computer screen, before providing them with a brief preparation window, after which they are given a limited amount of time to provide an answer to the question. While going through these stages, participants view themselves on screen and their responses are recorded.
A screenshot of each component of the Interview Challenge can be found in Fig. 2. At the commencement of the interview task, the experimenter set up the interface, with the participant’s video inside the white box, enabling them to view themselves answering the interview questions. This self-evaluation was included to increase task-related anxiety (Righi et al., 2009). Next, participants were presented with an instruction screen that introduced the format of the preparation component of the Interview Challenge. Participants were told they would be presented with an interview question and given one minute to prepare to answer this question, with a yellow progress bar indicating the elapsed time.
Following these instructions, participants were presented with the first question and given one minute to prepare. Once this one minute had elapsed, a second instruction screen was presented, which introduced the format of the answering component of the Interview Challenge. Participants were told the recording of their answer would commence on the next screen and they would be given two minutes to answer the question, which would be indicated using a red progress bar. On the next screen, participants were recorded for two minutes while they answered the question. The Interview Challenge comprised four questions, all of which followed the format described above (e.g., What are the three levels of government in Australia and what are their respective roles?). Further detail pertaining to question development can be found in the Online Supplementary Material. Interview and speech-based paradigms are used frequently within the literature to study anxiety symptomatology (e.g., Chen et al., 2016).

Post Stressor Breathing Focus Task

The purpose of this task was to facilitate assessment of anxiety perseveration in the period following the Interview Challenge. A task was required to provide an appropriate window for participants to experience anxiety perseveration in the wake of the interview challenge stressor. Adapted from Levinson and colleagues (2012) Breath Awareness Task, this nine-minute task was designed to have low working memory demand, to allow the cognitive capacity to think about the Interview Challenge. It has been identified by Levinson and colleagues (2012) that task-unrelated thoughts (such as those relating to the interview challenge) increase when a task makes minimal demand on working memory, thus increasing working memory capacity. Choosing a task requiring significant working memory capacity (e.g., operation span task; Turner & Engle, 1989) would thus likely have an inhibitory effect, restricting attention and thoughts to the task currently being completed (Levinson et al., 2012). This would make it challenging to assess perseveration, which requires individuals to have the capacity to consider the past stressor, especially higher-level considerations, including the reasons for and consequences of the event. Participants were instructed “Be aware of the movement of your breath, breathe normally and with each exhale, press the letter E”. “If you suddenly realize that your attention was completely off task, that’s ok. Press the control button, and gently bring the attention back to your breath”. Throughout this nine-minute task, state anxiety was assessed every three minutes, using the visual analogue state anxiety measure.

Procedure

Participants were invited to a study about individual differences in information processing and the effect on emotion and behaviour. They completed the study individually in sound-attenuated rooms, in a quiet laboratory setting at the University of Western Australia. The experimental session commenced with participants providing informed written consent, before completing a brief demographic information questionnaire. Following this, participants completed a battery of questionnaires, which included the STAI-T, and the baseline state anxiety measure, which commenced the anxiety reactivity assessment phase. This initial phase of the experimental session ran, on average, for 30 min.
Participants then viewed information, which introduced the Interview Challenge stressor, before completing the stressor introduction state anxiety measure. Instructions for the video clip task were subsequently presented on screen, before participants were exposed to four videos from the category they had been assigned to, based on the order of enrolment within the study. As each video ran for approximately one minute, this task ran for a total of approximately four minutes. Once they had viewed these videos, participants completed the post video clips state anxiety measure, which concluded the anxiety reactivity assessment phase.
Participants next completed the Interview Challenge stressor, which ran for approximately 12 min, and which was followed by the post interview state anxiety assessment, which commenced the anxiety perseveration assessment phase. The final component of the experimental session was the breathing focus task, which took nine minutes to complete and required participants to report on their state anxiety every three minutes throughout the nine-minute task. These measurements formed the breath task state anxiety measurements one, two, and three. At the end of the breath task, the perseveration assessment phase concluded (see Fig. 3 for an overview of the lab-based experimental session).

Assessment of Anxiety Reactivity and Anxiety Perseveration

During the anxiety reactivity assessment phase, anxiety reactivity was measured through the increase observed between two critical state anxiety measurements. During the anxiety perseveration assessment phase, anxiety perseveration was measured by averaging scores on the four state anxiety measurements that occurred throughout this phase.
As such, the visual analogue state anxiety measure was included throughout the experimental session (see Fig. 3 for exact timepoints) to facilitate this assessment of anxiety reactivity, and anxiety perseveration. There were seven assessment points overall, one which served as a baseline measure of state anxiety, two which served as reactivity assessment points, and four which served as perseveration assessment points.

Assessing Anxiety Reactivity

The first reactivity assessment point was taken after participants were introduced to the Interview Challenge stressor and informed they would complete this interview during the second half of the experimental session. This assessment point will be referred to as the stressor introduction state anxiety assessment. The second reactivity assessment point was taken after participants were exposed to the four video clips. This assessment point will be referred to as the post video clips state anxiety assessment.

Assessing Anxiety Perseveration

The initial perseveration assessment point was taken after participants completed the Interview Challenge. This assessment point will be referred to as the post interview state anxiety assessment point. Following this assessment point, participants completed the breath awareness perseveration task, during which, three further measurements of state anxiety were taken at three-minute intervals throughout this task. These assessment points will be referred to as breath task state anxiety assessment one, two, and three.

Results

Data Analytic Plan

In order to test the proposed influence of processing of situational information on anxiety reactivity and processing of implicational information on anxiety perseveration, mixed-design ANOVAs were conducted using the data analytic software SPSS 29 (IBM Corp., 2023). More specifically, for the anxiety reactivity hypothesis, a 2 × 2 × 2 mixed design ANOVA was employed on state anxiety scores (DV), with the between subject factors of Valence (IV) and Information Style (IV) and within subjects factor of Time (IV). For the anxiety perseveration hypothesis, a 4 × 2 × 2 mixed-design ANOVA was employed on state anxiety scores (DV), with the between subject factors of Valence (IV) and Information Style (IV) and within subjects factor of Time (IV). Supplementary analyses, including assessment of the unique contribution of anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration indices to trait anxiety scores, were investigated using multiple regression analyses.

Data Preparation

Two participants did not complete the study and so were excluded. Outliers were removed based on the Mahalanobis Distance for the detection of multivariate outliers (Aguinis et al., 2013). Seven variables were entered into the outlier analysis, corresponding to the state anxiety measurements across both the anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration assessment periods. Distances were interpreted using p <.001 and corresponding chi-square values with the degrees of freedom equal to the number of variables entered into the analysis (see Dashdondov and Kim (2021) for additional methodological details concerning outlier detection using Mahalanobis Distance). Based on this methodology, six additional participants were excluded from the final data set, resulting in a final sample size of 134.
As the aim of the present study was to determine how exposure to information differing in information type and valence effects participants’ anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration, it was important to isolate the effects of exposure to this information, without the influence of participants’ baseline tendency to experience elevations in state anxiety. Therefore, preliminary analyses were conducted to compare participants’ trait anxiety, as assessed by the STAI-T, prior to taking part in the experimental tasks. An exploratory 2 × 2 between-subjects ANOVA, compared STAIT-T scores across the between-subjects factors of Valence (Positive vs. Negative) and Information Type (Situational vs. Implicational). Results revealed a significant main effect of Information Type F(1, 130) = 4.10, p =.045, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.03\), with participants in the Situational conditions having significantly higher (M = 69.23, SE = 9.46) STAI-T scores than those in the implicational conditions (M = 65.56, SD = 11.40) (see Table 1 for detailed STAI-T descriptive statistics).
Table 1
Mean, SD, Minimum, and Maximum STAI-T scores in each Condition and the total sample
Condition
Mean
SD
Minimum
Maximum
Total
47.40
10.60
21.00
67.00
Situational Negative
49.90
9.17
33.00
67.00
Situational Positive
48.60
9.89
22.00
62.00
Implicational Negative
47.00
11.70
28.00
67.00
Implicational Positive
44.10
11.10
21.00
65.00

Success of Intended Anxiety Induction by Interview Challenge

To determine whether the introduction to the Interview Challenge was effective at inducing elevated state anxiety, mean state anxiety scores at the baseline and stressor introduction measurement points were subjected to a paired samples t-test. Results revealed a significant difference between participants’ anxiety scores at the baseline measurement point (M = 38.1, SD = 28.2) and the stressor introduction (M = 62.0, SD = 26.9) measurement point t(133) = 11.3, p <.001, d = 0.98, indicating that the introduction to the Interview Challenge induced significant elevations in state anxiety.

Did Processing of Negative Situational Information Uniquely Impact Anxiety Reactivity?

Mean state anxiety scores at the Stressor Introduction and Post Video Clips measurement points were subjected to a 2 × 2 × 2 mixed-design ANOVA, with the between-subjects factors of Valence (Positive vs. Negative) and Information Type (Situational vs. Implicational), the within-subjects factor of Time (Stressor Introduction vs. Post Video Clips), and the STAI-T covariate. The hypothesis under test predicts that we should find a significant Information Type × Valence × Time interaction, due to participants who have been exposed to negative situational information exhibiting a disproportionately greater increase in state anxiety, when compared to those who have been exposed to negative implicational or positive information.
Results revealed no significant main effect of Time F(1, 129) = 3.37, p =.069, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.03.\:\)A significant interaction between Valence and Time F(1, 129) = 6.50, p =.012, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.05\:\)was identified. Follow up analyses revealed that following the presentation of the video clips, individuals who received positive information experienced a significant reduction in state anxiety, t(64) = 5.38, p <.001, d = 0.67, conversely, participants who received negative information did not experience a significant reduction in state anxiety t(68) = 0.77, p =.45, d = 0.09.
Critically, results revealed no significant interaction between Information Type, Valence, and Time F(1, 129) = 0.04, p =.84, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.00.\:\:\text{A}\text{d}\text{i}\text{t}\text{i}\text{o}\text{n}\text{a}\text{l}\text{l}\text{y},\:\)no significant interactions were identified between Information Type and Time F(1, 129) = 2.16, p =.144, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.02\), nor Information Type and Valence F(1, 129) = 0.99, p =.32, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.01,\:\)Similarly, there were no further significant main effects; Information Type F(1, 129) = 0.08, p =.78, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.00\), and Valence F(1, 129) = 0.95, p =.33, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.01.\)

Did Processing of Negative Implicational Information Uniquely Impact Anxiety Perseveration?

Mean state anxiety scores across the four perseveration time points were subjected to a 4 × 2 × 2 mixed-design ANOVA with the between-subjects factors of Valence (Positive vs. Negative) and Information Type (Situational vs. Implicational), the within-subjects factor of Time (Post Interview, Breath Task 1, Breath Task 2, and Breath Task 3), and the STAI-T covariate. Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated χ2 (5) = 151.36, p <.001. Consequently, Huynh-Feldt corrected results are reported.
Results revealed a significant main effect of Time F(1.91, 246.63) = 6.05, p =.003, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\) 0.05, indicating that participants’ mean state anxiety significantly differed across the four measurement points in the perseveration assessment period. Specifically, state anxiety significantly decreased across each of the four assessment points throughout the anxiety perseveration assessment phase (see Table 2). Results revealed no significant interaction between Information Type and Time F(1.91, 246.63) = 1.93, p =.149, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.02\), indicating that participants experienced comparable levels of state anxiety at each measurement point throughout the perseveration assessment period, irrespective of the information type they were exposed to. Additionally, there was no significant interaction between Time and Valence F(1.91, 246.63) = 2.52, p =.085, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.02,\) Information Type and Valence F(1, 129) = 0.35, p =.55, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.00\), nor a significant three-way interaction between Time, Information Type, and Valence F(1.91, 246.63) = 1.52, p =.22, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.01.\) Similarly, there were no further significant main effects; Information Type F(1, 129) = 0.32, p =.57, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.00\), and Valence F(1, 129) = 0.08, p =.78, \(\:{{\eta\:}_{p}}^{2}=\:0.00.\)
Table 2
Mean (and SD) state anxiety scores in each condition across perseveration assessment points
Assessment point
Implicational negative
Implicational positive
Situational negative
Situational positive
Post Interview
44.91(34.18)
38.26(29.50)
37.80(29.02)
45.26(26.27)
Breath Task 1
26.97(25.28)
22.09(22.38)
29.83(26.09)
28.68(20.27)
Breath Task 2
21.24(22.82)
14.41(16.45)
21.46(21.43)
17.61(18.50)
Breath Task 3
15.97(19.69)
11.03(15.62)
18.57(23.12)
14.06(18.28)

Discussion

The present study served as an initial investigation into the cognitive mechanisms underlying anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration, two dissociable dimensions of trait anxiety. Drawing from literature focusing on rumination (Watkins et al., 2008) and emotional regulation (Kross et al., 2005, 2014), we hypothesised that processing of negative information which is situational in nature leads to greater anxiety reactivity (but not perseveration) in the lead up to a stressful event, and that processing negative information which is implicational in nature leads to greater anxiety perseveration (but not anxiety reactivity) in the wake of a stressful event.
Results revealed that processing the negative situational information contained in the video clips did not significantly influence participants’ experience of state anxiety during the anxiety reactivity assessment period. Thus, these findings do not support the first hypothesis under test. This result is inconsistent with the support for this theoretical account found in the emotional regulation literature, where it was identified enhanced processing of situational details concerning a stressful event led to greater intensity of negative emotions (Bornstein et al., 2020). This discrepancy could be explained by methodological variations between the present study and Bornstein and colleagues (2020) study. For one, the present study utilised pre-recorded videos to induce a processing mode, which constrained participants to viewing information that might not have been in line with how they would naturally process a stressful situation. In contrast, Bornstein and colleagues (2020) asked participants to draw from their own past experiences, to provide a retelling of a stressful interpersonal experience, which was then coded as either situational or implicational in construal (Bornstein et al., 2020). This difference may have impacted the level of psychological distance perceived by the participants, i.e., listening to others’ experiences, rather than drawing from their own, may have attenuated the intensity of the emotional experience (Kross, 2009). There is evidence to support this, with Verduyn and colleagues (2012) finding that the duration of negative emotions was decreased when individuals processed an event from a self-distanced perspective (seeing themselves as an observer), compared to individuals who processed the event from a self-immersed perspective (first-person).
The present results additionally revealed that the processing of negative implicational information, presented in the video clips, did not significantly influence participants’ experience of state anxiety during the anxiety perseveration assessment period. Therefore, the second hypothesis under test was not supported. This result contrasts with those found in the depressive rumination literature, which highlight the contribution of the implicational processing mode to depressed mood and ruminative cognitions (Kambara et al., 2019; Watkins & Teasdale, 2004). These prior works similarly utilised convenience samples of unselected undergraduate students (i.e., students were not selected for high trait anxiety), so it is not unreasonable to assume that enhanced processing of implicational information could influence the experience of anxiety perseveration in a subclinical sample. Therefore, authors examined potential methodological explanations that may have influenced the present study’s ability to detect significant effects. One candidate explanation for our non-significant effect could be that individuals did not experience substantial anxiety perseveration in the wake of the Interview Challenge stressor. This explanation is given weight by the finding that state anxiety significantly reduced across the anxiety perseveration assessment phase, irrespective of valence and information type contained in the videos. Perhaps this significant decrease in state anxiety during the perseveration assessment phase is due to a mismatch between expectancies formed during the reactivity assessment phase and the experience of the stressor itself. It is plausible that while participants’ state anxiety was elevated by both the introduction to the Interview Challenge stressor, the stressor itself did not match their expectations in terms of difficulty or arousal. This could have elicited feelings of surprise and relief (Gaschler et al., 2014), and a subsequent drop in state anxiety. Relief has been described a “tension reducing emotion” (Pekrun et al., 2007 p. 25), which is linked to a reduction in subjective negative states, such as state anxiety. Research examining expectation mismatch and prediction error lend weight to this suggestion, as it has been identified that a mismatch between predictions and/or expectations of outcomes, and the actual outcome itself can influence performance, and the experience of anxiety (White et al., 2017; Gaschler et al., 2014).
A condition requiring processing of positive situational information, and a condition requiring processing of positive implicational information, were also included to disentangle the impact of processing situational vs. implicational information from the impact of processing negative vs. positive information. The valence of the video clips emerged as a factor which influenced participants’ anxiety reactivity. More specifically, while participants who processed positive information (situational or implicational) experienced a reduction in their level of state anxiety following their viewing of the video clips, individuals who were exposed to negative information remained elevated in their state anxiety ratings after viewing the video clips. This finding is consistent with a significant body of research that implicates the processing of negative information in the experience of heightened state anxiety and anxiety related dysfunction (Bar-Haim et al., 2007; Mathews & MacLeod, 2005). Of note, valence of processed information influenced only anxiety reactivity, without having any impact on anxiety perseveration. Prior research into anxiety reactivity and perseveration has not considered the influence of valence on the experience of these independent dimensions (Rudaizky et al., 2012, 2014), making this a novel finding. This finding will be important to consider when aiming to target elevated anxiety, as exposure to positive information may only attenuate anxiety reactivity, and not anxiety perseveration.
As an additional endeavour, this research sought to conduct analyses akin to those presented in Rudaizky and colleagues’ (2014) study. These analyses aimed to explore the ability of in vivo measures of anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration to explain independent variance in trait anxiety scores. The study found that only anxiety perseveration accounted for independent variance in trait anxiety scores, contrasting with Rudaizky et al., (2014), but aligning with Mann (2020), who also identified anxiety perseveration as the sole independent predictor. Differences in experimental design may explain the discrepancy; Rudaizky et al.‘s (2014) study used a stressor where participants anticipated a speech without delivering it, while the current study and Mann’s methodology involved a simulated interview or speech with preparation time. Furthermore, Rudaizky and colleagues (2014) allowed more time for participants to experience anxiety reactivity, by assessing state anxiety across a five-minute window. In contrast, the present study assessed participants’ state anxiety immediately after exposing them to the video clips, providing less opportunity for anxiety reactivity to develop.
Although the present design did elicit elevations in state anxiety during the anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration assessment phases, future studies could modify the study protocol, with the goal of increasing the degree to which participants experience anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration. For example, such refinements may involve amending the questions involved in the interview, to reflect topics of relevance to undergraduate samples (e.g., housing, climate change, and education). Increasing the personal relevance of experimental stimuli to undergraduate participants could be a pathway to increased engagement with the stressor (Grafton et al., 2012), and greater negative affectivity, characteristic of anxiety perseveration (Grafton et al., 2012). Additionally, it could be the case that the lack of anxiety reactivity effects in the present study were at least in part driven by a ceiling effect, occurring as a result of the stressor introduction. It is plausible that participants anticipation of the Interview Challenge stressor elevated anxiety to a point that eclipsed any further elevation that could have been elicited by the information contained in the videos. To avoid this, future experimental designs should endeavour to assess anxiety in a manner that acknowledges the contribution of the stressor introduction and the subsequent exposure to information concerning the interview, to provide a holistic measure of reactivity. By including both in the measure of anxiety reactivity, any ceiling effect will be accounted for.
Despite its strengths, the present study has several limitations of note. First, the current study did not recruit participants based on their level of trait anxiety, instead electing to recruit a convenience sample of undergraduates. It could be the case that impact of processing situational information and implicational information, on anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration respectively, may be more evident in people with high levels of trait anxiety. The design of the present study precluded the detection of effects that may only exist for participants with high levels of trait anxiety. Future research could overcome this limitation by recruiting participants using a measure of trait anxiety, such as the STAI-T (Spielberger, 1983), to enable comparing the impact of processing situational information and implicational information on anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration experienced by participants high in trait anxiety, compared to participants low in trait anxiety.
An additional limitation is that the present study was not able to determine the natural processing style of participants, which may vary across individuals in terms of whether this involves a preference for, or tendency to selectively process, either situational information or implicational information. Instead, individuals in the current study were presented with pre-selected information, which was either situational or implicational in nature, and so the information they were required to process may not have aligned with their own information processing preferences. In line with this proposal, works by Watkins and colleagues (2008, 2020) have identified that individuals who experience greater depressive rumination have an inherent tendency to process information relating to the causes, consequences, and implications of negative mood and when provided with incongruent, often concrete information, they experience reduced duration of negative mood and ruminative cognitions (Watkins & Roberts, 2020). For the present investigation, it could be the case that some individuals who have a tendency to preferentially process situational information were given implicational information, which may have led to diminished anxiety reactivity and/or perseveration (and vice versa) with the implicational processing style. Future extensions of this work could employ methodology that permits identification of participants’ information processing biases (e.g., by asking participants to report on the contents of their thoughts). This could help uncover individual differences in information processing biases which may drive preferential processing of either situational or implicational information, potentially resulting in differing levels of anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration. Hence, we suggest that this is an important future direction for research in this field.
Finally, mention must be made to sample characteristics, most notably, the current sample consisting of mostly young adults from a western, educated, industrialised, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) society (Wild et al., 2022). Credible concerns have been raised about the external validity of research conducted using undergraduate samples, due to the fact these individuals are rarely representative of the general population (Wild et al., 2022). Such validity concerns relate to the narrow age range, sophisticated cognitive abilities and/or skills, and higher socioeconomic backgrounds of these samples, compared to the general population (Wild et al., 2022). With these consideration in mind, the authors recommend exercising caution when considering generalising the current findings to populations outside of university undergraduate samples.
To summarise, in the present study the processing of negative situational and negative implicational information about an upcoming stressor did not serve to differentially influence the degree to which participants exhibited anxiety reactivity and anxiety perseveration in response to this stressor. Nonetheless, these findings build on previous work by Watkins (2008) and Kross (2005, 2014), as this is the first study to examine the influence of exposure to information which is either abstract and decontextualised in nature (implicational) or distinct and situation specific in nature (situational), on the two distinct facets of trait anxiety. From this foundational study, we hope further research will be conducted to illuminate the information processing biases implicated in the experience of anxiety reactivity and perseveration, furthering our theoretical, and applied understanding of these dimensions of trait anxiety. Once these have been identified, it opens the possibility for not only determining whether this relationship is causal, but for developing paradigms capable of targeting information processing biases (e.g., attention bias) with the aim of reducing individuals’ experience of anxiety reactivity and/or anxiety perseveration. One such paradigm could incorporate findings from the literature base, which highlight the benefits of training individuals to attend to and process positive information instead of negative information, in the reduction of general distress and/or anxiety symptoms (Blanco et al., 2023; Sîrbu et al., 2025). This has the potential to extend our novel valence finding, by encouraging the development of a positive information processing bias, through the use of an online bias modification paradigm, with the goal of attenuating the experience of anxiety reactivity.
These modification-based interventions are often developed to supplement treatment as usual, or to reach individuals who are currently on waitlists for psychological intervention (Vrijsen et al., 2024). While there have been queries raised as to whether these interventions are effective in modifying cognitive biases (Jones & Sharpe, 2017), a novel intervention could be designed which incorporates proposed amendments and addresses common criticisms, as the benefits of these often online based interventions are hard to overlook. Firstly, they would allow researchers to modify relatively automatic processes, which is hard to achieve through other cognitive interventions, and the digital format would allow engagement for populations who are often difficult to reach (e.g. those living rurally) (Vrijsen et al., 2024). Developing and piloting a novel intervention could prove beneficial in targeting the two dimensions of trait anxiety separately, making this proposal particularly relevant for individuals with mental health concerns which likely map onto one dimension more than the other (e.g., panic disorder and anxiety reactivity; Rudaizky et al., 2014).

Declarations

All individuals whose likeness appear in this manuscript have given written consent for publication for research purposes. The images are the copyright of the University of Western Australia.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

BSL GOP_opleiding GZ-psycholoog

Bijlagen

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (2005). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. Basic Books/Hachette Book Group. Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (2005). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. Basic Books/Hachette Book Group.
go back to reference Blanco, I., Boemo, T., Martin-Garcia, O., Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., & Sanchez-Lopez, A. (2023). Online contingent attention training (OCAT): Transfer effects to cognitive biases, rumination, and anxiety symptoms from two proof-of-principle studies. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 8(1), 28. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-023-00480-3CrossRefPubMed Blanco, I., Boemo, T., Martin-Garcia, O., Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., & Sanchez-Lopez, A. (2023). Online contingent attention training (OCAT): Transfer effects to cognitive biases, rumination, and anxiety symptoms from two proof-of-principle studies. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 8(1), 28. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s41235-023-00480-3CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Julian L. J. (2011). Measures of anxiety: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-Anxiety (HADS-A). Arthritis care & research, 63 Suppl 11(0 11), S467–S472. https://doi.org/10.1002/acr.20561 Julian L. J. (2011). Measures of anxiety: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-Anxiety (HADS-A). Arthritis care & research, 63 Suppl 11(0 11), S467–S472. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​acr.​20561
go back to reference Pekrun, R., Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., & Perry, R. P. (2007). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: An integrative approach to emotions in education. In P. Schutz, & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotion in Education (pp. 13–36). Academic. Pekrun, R., Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., & Perry, R. P. (2007). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: An integrative approach to emotions in education. In P. Schutz, & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotion in Education (pp. 13–36). Academic.
go back to reference Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (form Y). Consulting Psychologists. Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (form Y). Consulting Psychologists.
go back to reference Vrijsen, J. N., Grafton, B., Koster, E. H. W., Lau, J., Wittekind, C. E., Bar-Haim, Y., Becker, E. S., Brotman, M. A., Joormann, J., Lazarov, A., MacLeod, C., Manning, V., Pettit, J. W., Rinck, M., Salemink, E., Woud, M. L., Hallion, L. S., & Wiers, R. W. (2024). Towards implementation of cognitive bias modification in mental health care: State of the science, best practices, and ways forward. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 179, 104557. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2024.104557CrossRefPubMed Vrijsen, J. N., Grafton, B., Koster, E. H. W., Lau, J., Wittekind, C. E., Bar-Haim, Y., Becker, E. S., Brotman, M. A., Joormann, J., Lazarov, A., MacLeod, C., Manning, V., Pettit, J. W., Rinck, M., Salemink, E., Woud, M. L., Hallion, L. S., & Wiers, R. W. (2024). Towards implementation of cognitive bias modification in mental health care: State of the science, best practices, and ways forward. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 179, 104557. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​brat.​2024.​104557CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
The Role of Information Processing as an Underlying Mechanism in the Experience of Anxiety Reactivity and Anxiety Perseveration, Two Dissociable Dimensions of Trait Anxiety
Auteurs
Sophia Moore
Laura Dondzilo
Mahdi Mazidi
Colin MacLeod
Lies Notebaert
Publicatiedatum
25-01-2025
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Cognitive Therapy and Research
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-025-10576-3