Skip to main content

Welkom bij Scalda & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Scalda heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen.Je kunt de producten hieronder links aanschaffen en rechts inloggen.

Registreer

Schaf de BSL Academy aan: 

BSL Academy mbo AG

Eenmaal aangeschaft kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met Jan van der Velden.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top

Open Access 02-04-2025 | Original Article

Uncovering the Temporal Dynamics of Negative Thought in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Auteurs: Cameron Pugach, Shae Nester, Blair Wisco

Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Purpose

Cognitive theory posits that negative posttraumatic thoughts play a critical role in the development, maintenance, and treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Though negative thought in PTSD is often measured using static, between-subjects, and cross-sectional assessments via retrospective self-report, recent approaches have investigated negative thought as a dynamic process that unfolds within people over time. Here, we examine the temporal dynamics of negative thought in daily life (i.e., variability, inertia, and reactivity) and whether these dynamics are associated with PTSD severity in trauma-exposed adults.

Methods

Participants with (n = 39) and without (n = 41) a current PTSD diagnosis completed three days of ecological momentary assessment (n = 2158 observations; Mobs = 27) assessing four subdomains of negative thoughts.

Results

Participants reported variability in negative thought over time, and that variability was explained by both situational and dispositional factors. Higher PTSD severity was associated with higher mean levels, more variability, and more reactivity in negative thought over time, but not negative thought inertia.

Conclusions

Findings suggest that negative thought is a dynamic process that exhibits short-term fluctuations and that the temporal dynamics of negative thought help characterize the cognitive experience of PTSD. Future work should incorporate ambulatory assessment and interventions to better understand and intervene on negative thought in PTSD.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10608-025-10608-y.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Negative posttraumatic thought plays a critical role in the development, maintenance, and treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Cognitive models of PTSD describe how trauma can contribute to negative thought that maintains PTSD symptoms (Ehlers & Clark, 2000; Foa et al., 2006; Resick & Schnicke, 1992). For example, difficulty assimilating one’s trauma with preexisting “just world” beliefs (e.g., good things happen to good people) can result in dysfunctional conclusions (e.g., “I must have deserved what happened”) or conversely, over-accommodated changes to the belief system (e.g., “nobody can be trusted”). Negative thought in turn promotes trauma-related avoidance behaviors that reinforce dysfunctional beliefs. Cognitive theories have also identified different types of negative thoughts that maintain PTSD, such as negative thoughts about oneself (e.g., self-blame, negative self-attributions) and others and the world (e.g., other-blame, negative other- and world-attributions). There is convincing support for cognitive models of PTSD. Negative cognitive processes, including negative thought and maladaptive cognitive styles such as rumination (i.e., the tendency to passively perseverate on the causes, meaning, and consequences of one’s negative emotions and symptoms; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008), are robustly associated with PTSD diagnosis and severity (Brown et al., 2019; Losavio et al., 2017; Moulds et al., 2020).
Several trauma-focused treatments for PTSD, including first-line interventions, were founded on cognitive theories (e.g., Cognitive Processing Therapy [CPT], Resick et al., 2017; Cognitive Therapy for PTSD; Ehlers et al., 2005). These treatments aim to reduce PTSD symptoms by helping patients challenge negative thoughts or decrease unhelpful cognitive processes (e.g., rumination) to promote more balanced cognitions that facilitate healthier emotional responses. Cognitive interventions are effective in treating PTSD with moderate to large effects (Cusack et al., 2016) and reductions in negative thought are a central mechanism of PTSD symptom change (Dillon et al., 2020; Holliday et al., 2018; Schumm et al., 2015). Indeed, a systematic review of mechanisms of change in clinically recommended treatments for PTSD found that reduction in negative thought was the most consistent mechanism of symptom change (Alpert et al., 2023), providing compelling support for the role of cognitive change in PTSD symptom improvement.
Extant research on negative thought in trauma-exposed adults is disproportionately based on retrospective self-report measures that ask people how much they agree with statements about negative posttraumatic thought (Brown et al., 2019). Perhaps the most widely used instrument is the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI; Foa et al., 1999), which asks respondents to report how much they agree with statements about negative posttraumatic thoughts such as self-blame and negative thoughts about the self and the world. On the one hand, use of retrospective self-report measures of negative thought such as the PTCI has generated reliable evidence and facilitated cross-study comparisons on general tendencies (i.e., differences between people) in negative thought. On the other hand, these methods are limited by asking people to introspect on their general thought patterns over ambiguous timeframes. Further, they are unable to unpack how negative thought manifests during specific moments (i.e., how negative thought unfolds within people over time and how these patterns are influenced by characteristics like PTSD severity).

Dynamic Approaches to Negative Thought

Following application in affective science (Houben et al., 2015; Kuppens & Verduyn, 2017; Reitsema et al., 2022), there is increased focus on understanding patterns of temporal fluctuation—or temporal dynamics (Hamaker & Wichers, 2017)—of negative thought. In these studies, negative thoughts are repeatedly measured within people using intensive longitudinal methods (e.g., ecological momentary assessment) and temporal dynamics are then extracted from the data. Temporal dynamics of negative thought have provided insights into the cognitive experiences of depression and anxiety (Bean & Ciesla, 2024; Bean et al., 2020, 2021; Blanke et al., 2022; Cole et al., 2021; Kiekens et al., 2024). To the best of our knowledge, however, no study has yet examined the temporal dynamics of negative posttraumatic thought and its relationships to PTSD. Examining the temporal dynamics of negative posttraumatic thought might provide insight into the cognitive experience of PTSD by clarifying how much negative thought fluctuates over time, revealing patterns in these fluctuations, and understanding how these patterns might serve as unique markers for PTSD severity.
Different measures of temporal dynamics exist, some of which add more unique explanatory information to the prediction of well-being than others (Dejonckheere et al., 2019). Three commonly studied dynamics are variability (i.e., how much a psychological process deviates from one’s mean level, measured using the person-specific standard deviation), inertia (i.e., how much a process self-perpetuates or resists change over time, measured using autocorrelation or autoregressive effects), and reactivity (i.e., change in a process shifts in concert with or in response to stress) (Kuppens & Verduyn, 2017). Investigations of cognitive variability have focused on rumination. Findings show that ruminative variability is elevated in adults with past-year non-suicidal self-injury (Kiekens et al., 2024) and higher depressive and social anxiety symptoms (Bean & Ciesla, 2024). This latter study further showed that ruminative variability prospectively predicted depression and anxiety symptoms three months later, indicating that greater departures from average levels of rumination might portend later psychopathology.
Investigations on cognitive inertia have also focused on rumination, given that rumination is often described as a self-perpetuating process of becoming “cognitively stuck” (Koval et al., 2012). Findings show that higher ruminative inertia is both a trigger and a response to negative affect (Blanke et al., 2022), in line with theoretical models of rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Further, ruminative inertia is associated with higher depressive symptoms in clinical (Bean et al., 2020) and non-clinical samples (Bean et al., 2021), but does not predict symptom increases over time (Bean & Ciesla, 2024). These findings suggest that the tendency for rumination to self-perpetuate may be a marker of depression maintenance rather than onset.
Finally, following theories of cognitive vulnerability (Beck, 1967), negative thoughts are thought to be activated during stress. Cognitive reactivity characterizes individuals at risk for psychopathology (e.g., depression) and is associated with onset, maintenance, and relapse (Scher et al., 2005). One study examining cognitive reactivity in daily life found that individuals higher in depression exhibit more cognitive reactivity (i.e., negative thoughts about themselves, the future, and the world), although they measured reactivity to negative affect rather than stress (Cole et al., 2021). Findings suggests that daily fluctuation in cognitive reactivity is characteristic of depression, consistent with theories of cognitive vulnerability.

Negative Thought Dynamics in PTSD

Examining negative thought dynamics in PTSD might is important for several reasons. First, although longitudinal studies have found that PTSD is a chronic condition characterized by relative stability (Lee et al., 2020; Marmar et al., 2015), PTSD symptoms also vary on short timescales, including day-to-day and hour-to-hour (Bridges-Curry, 2025; Greene, 2021; Reeves & Fisher, 2020). Because negative thoughts are part of the symptom constellation of PTSD (i.e., negative beliefs about oneself, others, and the world [Criterion D2], self- and other-blame [Criterion D3]; Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5-TR; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2022), it follows that negative thought may also vary on short timescales. Further, research on the temporal course of PTSD symptoms suggests that negative posttraumatic cognitions and emotions are central symptoms to PTSD and strong predictors of variation in other symptoms over time (Bridges-Curry, 2025; Reeves & Fisher, 2020) Understanding the degree to which negative thought is a static versus dynamic component of PTSD could help inform conceptualization and treatment efforts.
Second, despite consistent findings that overall levels of negative posttraumatic thought are elevated in PTSD and represent a mechanism of change in trauma-focused psychotherapies, research has also found that certain subdomains of negative thought are differentially related to PTSD risk and treatment. For example, the PTCI reflects not only the overall degree of negative thought that one espouses, but breaks down negative thought into subdomains of self-blame, negative self-attributions, and negative other- and world-attributions (Foa et al., 1999). Some research has shown that changes in negative thoughts about the self (i.e., self-blame, negative self-attributions) predict PTSD symptom change in CPT (Schumm et al., 2015), whereas others have found that changes in self-blame predicted PTSD symptom change in CPT (Dillon et al., 2020; Holliday et al., 2018). Still, another study found that changes in negative self-, other-, and world-attributions predicted symptom change in PE (Kumpula et al., 2017). Though it is difficult to draw conclusions from these conflicting results, they suggest that (1) subdomains of negative thought might be uniquely responsive to treatment and that (2) different people may struggle with different subdomains of negative thought. Thus, examining the dynamics of negative thought overall and at the subdomain level is important.
Finally, alterations in the temporal dynamics of negative thought—namely, variability, inertia, and reactivity—might be important markers that provide insight into PTSD. The DSM-5-TR defines negative thoughts in PTSD as persistent and exaggerated (APA, 2022), which hint at the dynamic properties of these symptoms. For example, persistence infers resistance to change over time, which can be captured via inertia. Exaggeration reflects increased frequency and intensity of negative thought, providing more opportunity for variability in negative thought over time and particularly during moments when negative thought is “triggered” by environmental stress, consistent with cognitive reactivity (Beck, 1967). Though properties of variability and stability seem mutually opposing, meta-analytic research shows that psychological dysfunction is associated with emotions that are both more variable and more inert (Houben et al., 2015). This pattern of dynamics suggests that psychological dysfunction is characterized by larger and more extreme emotional shifts that once activated take longer to dissipate. Together with research documenting links between these cognitive dynamics and other forms of psychopathology, it is theoretically and empirically plausible that these dynamics might also emerge in PTSD.

The Present Study

Consequently, this study sought to use ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to examine the temporal dynamics of negative thought in a sample of trauma-exposed adults—approximately half of whom met criteria for a current PTSD diagnosis—and to test whether individual differences in these characteristics are associated with PTSD severity. PTSD was modeled dimensionally because symptom severity scales demonstrate better reliability and validity than categorical diagnoses (Shankman et al., 2018) and because recent work suggests that PTSD is likely best represented as a dimensional construct (Klein et al., 2024). We posed four primary hypotheses. First, we expected that PTSD severity would be positively associated with mean levels of momentary negative thought, consistent with the larger trait literature (H1). Second, we expected that variability in negative thought would be predominantly explained by state (i.e., within-person) rather than dispositional (i.e., between-subjects) factors, reflecting the time-varying nature of negative thought, and that PTSD severity would be positively associated with variability in negative thought in daily life (H2). Third, we expected that negative thought would be inert or resistant to change across time, and that inertia would be higher in people with more severe PTSD (H3). Fourth, to index reactivity, we hypothesized that perceived stress would predict concurrent shifts in negative thought, and that these shifts would be greater in people with more severe PTSD (H4).
We tested all hypotheses using a composite that includes four subdomains of negative thought often studied in PTSD: Self-Blame, Other-Blame, Negative Self-Attributions, and Negative Other- and World-Attributions. These types of negative thought were selected to mirror commonly used self-report measures (e.g., PTCI; Foa et al., 1999) and DSM-5 symptoms. We should note that the level of cognition examined here (i.e., negative thought) refers to surface-level thoughts that are negatively valenced and readily accessible to participants. We do not label instances of negative thought as distorted because that would require a level of contextual information that is difficult to achieve with the brief, repeated nature of daily life assessment. Due to possible differences in negative thought type, we examined subdomains of negative thought by separately analyzing Self-Blame, Other-Blame, Negative Self-Attributions, and Negative Other- and World-attributions. We considered these analyses fully exploratory. Findings from the current study are expected to inform the cognitive underpinnings of PTSD and the development of ambulatory interventions to address negative thoughts in real time.

Method

This study was a non-preregistered secondary data analysis of an existing dataset from the Ambulatory Psychophysiology of PTSD Study. All measures and data collected will be available to the general research community from the National Data Archive following an embargo period at the end date of this research award. An a priori power analysis conducted for the parent project determined the target sample size. We used the largest available sample size and report data exclusions in the “Participants” subsection. All procedures were approved by the University of North Carolina at Greensboro Institutional Review Board.

Participants

Eighty-five trauma-exposed, adult community members were recruited from a mid-sized city in the Southeastern United States for a study examining the impact of trauma reminders on psychophysiology in daily life. Eligible participants were English-speaking, 18–40 years with a body mass index (BMI) between 18.5 and 34.9, and had lifetime exposure to at least one traumatic event consistent with DSM-5 (APA, 2022). BMI was an inclusion criteria due to physiological data collected as part of the parent study that are not analyzed here. Exclusion criteria were past-month trauma exposure, psychosis, and factors known to affect physiological data acquisition including dissociation, pregnancy, cardiovascular disease, and medications known to affect cardiovascular functioning (e.g., antidepressants, antihistamines, blood pressure medication). Five participants were excluded due to current antidepressant use (n = 1), active suicidal ideation (n = 2), loss to follow up before EMA (n = 1), or withdrawal from the study (n = 1).
The final sample included 80 participants– 41 trauma-exposed controls (TECs) and 39 participants who met criteria for a current PTSD diagnosis based on the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5; Weathers et al., 2013a). Demographic characteristics are shown in Table 1. Briefly, participants were M = 21.79 years old (SD = 4.21), 75% female, and completed at least some college (51.2%). Participants were: 35% White; 28.7% Black; 21.3% Hispanic/Latino; 7.5% Multiracial/Other; 5% Biracial; and 2.5% Asian/Pacific Islander.
Table 1
Sample characteristics and current (past-month) psychiatric comorbidity
Variable
Full sample (N = 80)
Association with PTSD Severity
Demographic information
Age (M, SD)
21.79 (4.21)
r =  − 0.17, p = 0.141
Gender (n, % female)
60 (75.0%)
t(78) = 0.02, p = 0.987
Race (n, %)
 
F(5,74) = 1.07, p = .386
  White (Not hispanic)
28 (35.0%)
 
  Black/African American
23 (28.7%)
 
  Hispanic/Latino
17 (21.3%)
 
  Asian/Pacific Islander
2 (2.5%)
 
  Biracial
4 (5.0%)
 
  Multiracial/Other
6 (7.5)
 
Education (n, %)
 
F(4, 75) = 0.60, p = 0.993
  High school diploma
14 (17.5%)
 
  Associate’s degree
9 (11.3%)
 
  Some college
41 (51.2%)
 
  Bachelor’s degree
8 (10.0%)
 
  Graduate degree
7 (8.8%)
 
Religion (n, %)
 
F(4,75) = 0.57, p = 0.687
  Christian
41 (51.2%)
 
  Jewish
1 (1.3%)
 
  Hindu
1 (1.3%)
 
  Other
6 (7.5%)
 
  None
30 (37.5%)
 
Romantic relationship (n, % yes)
42 (52.5%)
t(77) =  − 1.59, p = 0.117
PTSD severity (CAPS-5; M, SD)
19.81 (12.07)
 
Psychiatric diagnosis (n, % yes)
  
 ≥ 1 psychiatric diagnosis
46 (60.5%)
t(75) =  − 4.43, p < 0.001
  Bipolar I disorder
7 (8.8%)
 
  Bipolar II disorder
4 (5.0%)
 
  Major depressive disorder
20 (25.0%)
 
  Persistent depressive disorder
7 (8.8%)
 
  Premenstrual dysphoric disorder
8 (10.0%)
 
  Agoraphobia
2 (2.5%)
 
  Social anxiety disorder
24 (30.0%)
 
  Specific phobia
9 (11.3%)
 
  Generalized anxiety disorder
24 (30.0%)
 
CAPS-5 = Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5. One participant did not report on education, religion, and romantic relationship status. Clinical interviews were not available for five participants. The independent samples t-test for comorbidity reflects the association between PTSD severity and the presence of at least one comorbid current psychiatric diagnosis

Procedures

Participants were recruited from the local community using online advertisements, flyers, and through a repository of previous research participants who consented to future contact. Participants completed an eligibility prescreening questionnaire using Qualtrics, which assessed for demographic and health-related information (e.g., medication use, BMI), psychosis (PRIME Screen; Miller, 2004), lifetime trauma history (Life Events Checklist for DSM-5, LEC-5; Weathers et al., 2013b), and past-month PTSD symptoms (Posttraumatic Checklist for DSM-5, PCL-5; Weathers et al., 2013c), including two questions to assess for the dissociative subtype (Herzog et al., 2020). Eligible respondents were then contacted via phone to confirm that the index (i.e., “worst”) trauma reported was at least one month ago and met for DSM-5 Criterion A trauma exposure (e.g., the event involved actual or threatened death, serious injury, or sexual violence; APA, 2022).
Eligible participants with “probable” PTSD were identified using a cutoff score of 33 on the PCL-5 (Bovin et al., 2016) and over-sampled to obtain an approximately 1:1 ratio of those with and without current PTSD. The PCL-5 was used only for sampling purposes; diagnostic status was confirmed with the CAPS-5 (described below). Recruitment was stratified based on age, sex, race/ethnicity, and trauma type (e.g., sexual assault, transportation accident, unexpected illness or death). The study was comprised of two laboratory sessions that flanked three days of ambulatory assessment, including EMA and ambulatory physiological assessment. In the first laboratory session, participants underwent structured clinical interviewing to assess for PTSD and comorbid DSM-5 mood and anxiety conditions. Next, participants completed three days of ambulatory assessment, described below. Finally, participants returned for a second laboratory session where they listened to two standardized study scripts as part of a script-driven imagery procedure and completed self-report questionnaires. Only the structured clinical interview and EMA data are analyzed here. Participants were compensated $150 USD after completing the study.
Within one week of the first laboratory session (M = 5.48 days, SD = 4.50), participants began the first of three days of ambulatory assessment. Ambulatory assessment days were often non-consecutive because participants had to come into the lab in the mornings to get outfitted with a mobile physiological acquisition device, which noninvasively and passively measures physiology using disposable electrodes placed on the skin. Participants were then oriented to the EMA survey administered using Qualtrics on a Lenovo tablet that was time-synced to the mobile device. Participants completed a practice survey and had the opportunity to ask questions. Participants then left the lab wearing the mobile equipment. Tablets were pseudo-randomly configured to administer surveys within 90-min blocks up to 17 times per day beginning at 9am and ending at approximately 11:30 pm, or when participants went to bed. The mean time between prompts was 53.67 min (SD = 13.34, range: 31–73). Participants were prompted by a tablet alarm at each survey to complete questions about their experiences in the past 10 min and had up to 20 min to respond. Participants removed the equipment at the end of each day and returned to the lab on the next scheduled day of assessment. The average number of days in between the first and third day of ambulatory assessment was 7.04 (SD = 4.36).

Measures

Structured Clinical Interview

PTSD The Clinician Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5; Weathers et al., 2013a) was used to determine current PTSD diagnosis and severity. The CAPS-5 is a structured clinical interview comprised of 20 items reflecting the DSM-5 PTSD symptom criteria. Items are coded for severity on a 5-point Likert scale (0 = Absent to 4 = Extreme/Incapacitating) based on the frequency and intensity of that symptom and are then summed to generate a PTSD severity score ranging from 0 to 80. Participants completed the CAPS-5 based on their past-month symptoms to assess current PTSD. All interviews were administered by a trained graduate student, audio-recorded, and independently scored by a second trained graduate student to determine reliability. Discrepancies were resolved through consensus with a doctoral-level psychologist with expertise in trauma and PTSD. Inter-rater reliability was ICC = 0.99.
Psychiatric Comorbidity The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5, Research Version (SCID-5-RV; First et al., 2015) was used to determine past and current psychiatric diagnoses. Modules A (Mood Episodes); D (Mood Disorders); and F (Anxiety Disorders) were administered. Diagnostic interviews were administered by a trained graduate student. A subset of interviews (n = 38) was coded by another trained graduate student to determine reliability. Inter-rater reliability ranged from ICC = 0.80–1.0.

Ecological Momentary Assessment

Negative Thought Participants rated four subdomains of negative thought at each observation. Participants were prompted with the instructions, “For the following questions, please keep in mind the trauma that the study has focused on so far. In the past 10 min…” and rated items on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 7 (Very much). The four items were: “I was blaming myself for the trauma” (Self-Blame), “I was blaming someone else for the trauma” (Other-Blame), “I was thinking negatively about myself” (Negative Self-Attributions), and “I was thinking negatively about other people or the world” (Negative Other- and World-Attributions). Items were selected to reflect DSM-5 symptoms and mirror frequently used measures of trauma-related negative beliefs (e.g., PTCI; Foa et al., 1999). Negative thought was measured in two ways. First, we summed all four items to create an overall composite of negative thought. McDonald’s Omega was used to calculate reliability for this composite within-(ωW = 0.66) and between-persons (ωB = 0.70). Second, we examined individual subdomains to assess differences in types of negative thought (see Supplement Table 1 for descriptive statistics).
Perceived Situational Stress At each survey, we measured participants’ appraisals of stressful situations using the single item, “In the past 10 min, my situation was stressful,” rated on the same 7-point Likert scale described above. Psychometric work suggests that though internal reliability cannot be estimated with a single item, single items of emotional experience show concurrent and predictive validity comparable to their multi-item counterparts in experience sampling designs (Song et al., 2023).

Analytic Strategy

We first assessed for associations between PTSD severity and demographic and clinical characteristics using bivariate correlations, independent samples t-tests, and analysis of variance (ANOVA). For main analyses, we examined mean levels (H1), variability (H2), inertia (H3), and reactivity (H4) in negative thought and their associations with PTSD severity (measured as total CAPS-5 score). Due to potential differences in subdomains of negative thought, we also conducted exploratory analyses by individually modeling Self-Blame, Other-Blame, Negative Self-Attributions, and Negative Other- and World-Attributions. Analyses were conducted in SPSS Version 28 and Mplus Version 8.7 using full-information maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors to handle missing data. This approach estimates parameters from the full sample with cases of incomplete data included in computations so that all available information is used in obtaining optimal parameter estimates (Arbuckle, 1996).
Variability in negative thought was computed using the person-specific SD1 (iSD) of negative thought across all observations, yielding one estimated score for each participant. PTSD-related differences in variability were assessed using Pearson’s correlation coefficients. For all other dynamics (i.e., mean levels, inertia, reactivity), we used multilevel models because of the nested nature of the data (Level 1 observations within Level 2 participants). We initially sought to treat the negative thought composite and all subdomains continuously. Examination of descriptive statistics and assumptions of multilevel regression models for Self-Blame and Other-Blame showed unacceptably high levels of skewness and kurtosis and non-normal distribution of residuals and random effects. After unsuccessful attempts at variable transformations, we dichotomized these two subdomains to reflect absence versus presence of blame. Scores of 1 = Not at all were coded as absent (0) and scores ≥ 2, indicating at least some blame, were coded as present (1). Thus, models were analyzed using multilevel linear regressions for the negative thought and negative attribution subdomains and multilevel logistic regressions for the blame subdomains. All output scripts for the main and subdomain multilevel models analyzed in Mplus and described below are available on the Open Science Framework: https://​osf.​io/​mvde5/​?​view_​only=​157d068605e044e4​b09d0a67d056b89c​
Multilevel modeling accounts for data dependencies and allows for disaggregation of within- and between-subjects effects while handling missing data and time-varying intervals between observations (Nezlek, 2012; Wang & Maxwell, 2015). An uncorrelated residual covariance structure was used for all multilevel models. Mean levels of negative thought were assessed as a means-as-outcomes model with PTSD severity entered at L2. Inertia was assessed using the autocorrelation in a slopes-as-outcomes model by regressing current negative thought (outcome) on negative thought at the previous observation (predictor) at L1, and whether PTSD severity (L2) impacts this relationship (cross-level interaction). Reactivity was assessed by testing if perceived situational stress is associated with concurrent shifts in negative thought using a slopes-as-outcomes model. In this model, we regressed current negative thought on situational stress at time t (L1), and if PTSD severity (L2) impacts this relationship (cross-level interaction). Negative thought at the last observation (t-1) was entered as another L1 predictor to adjust for prior levels of negative thought. For all lagged models, data were analyzed within-day such that the final assessment on one day was not used to predict the first assessment on the next EMA day. In all models, categorical and continuous L1 predictors were person-mean-centered and estimated as random effects and L2 predictors were grand-mean-centered (Yaremych et al., 2023). Robustness checks adjusting for person-specific mean levels of negative thought in analyses of negative thought variability, inertia, and reactivity were conducted to examine if effects were an artifact of individual differences in mean levels of negative thought (Dejonckheere et al., 2019). Finally, following suggestions from reviewers, we conducted two additional sets of robustness checks to determine if any significant cross-level effects of PTSD withstood covariation for the presence of a Major Depressive Disorder diagnosis, specifically, and at least one psychiatric disorder, generally. All significant findings reported below remained unchanged after robustness checks adjusting for the presence of psychiatric disorders. Thus, we opted to focus those models that solely include PTSD severity, adjusted for person-specific mean negative thought throughout all formulas, text, and tables.

Results

Sample Characteristics and Descriptive Statistics

Demographic and clinical characteristics for this sample have been previously reported but are reproduced here (Pugach et al., 2023). Consistent with our stratified recruitment procedures, PTSD severity did not differ in age, gender, race/ethnicity, education, religion, and romantic relationship status (see Table 1). The index trauma type breakdown in the full sample was sexual assault (42.5%); physical assault (23.8%); natural disaster, accident, or fire (16.3%); serious illness, injury, or death (16.3%); and combat exposure (1.3%). PTSD severity was not associated with index event type (F[4,75] = 0.83, p = 0.508), but PTSD severity was higher among participants with at least one comorbid psychiatric diagnosis (p < 0.001). In total, participants completed 2158 surveys (M = 26.98 surveys per person), or 63.1% of all surveys received (SD = 19.7). PTSD severity was not associated with survey completion rate, r =  − 0.04, p = 0.699.2
There are several descriptive findings worth highlighting (see Supplement Table 1). First, at least some negative thought was endorsed at 684 of 2158 observations (31.7%), with Self-Blame least frequently endorsed (n = 127, 5.9%) and Negative Attributions—Self most endorsed (n = 507, 23.5%). Second, the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) value in an unconditional multilevel model quantifies the proportion of variance between-subjects, with 1-ICC reflecting the proportion within-subjects. The ICC value of 0.43 showed that 43% of the variance in negative thought is between-person and 57% is within-person, suggesting that both state and dispositional factors influence variability in negative thought. Third, visual inspection of momentary fluctuations in negative thought showed that while people with higher PTSD severity reported higher average levels of negative thought than those with lower PTSD severity, most people experienced considerable variability in negative thought in daily life (Fig. 1). Finally, within- and between-persons correlations (Supplement Table 2) show that people who experience more negative thought also experience more perceived situational stress, and that moments characterized by increased negative thought are characterized by more perceived situational stress.

Mean Levels of Negative Thought (H1)

We next compared whether PTSD severity was associated with mean levels of negative thought. We estimated a means-as-outcomes model with PTSD severity (total CAPS-5 score) at L2 predicting overall negative thought:
Level 1:
$$ {\text{Negative Thought}}_{\left( t \right)ij} = \beta_{0j} + r_{ij} $$
Level 2:
$$ \beta_{0j} = \user2{ }\gamma_{00} + \user2{ }\gamma_{01} \left( {{\text{PTSD Severity}}_{j} } \right) + \mu_{0j} $$
The L1 β0j parameter represents each person’s average level of negative thought across observations. The γ00 parameter reflects negative thought at the average level of PTSD severity and the γ01 parameter represents the effect of deviations in PTSD severity on negative thought. Results showed a significant effect of PTSD severity on negative thought, b = 0.07, SE = 0.01, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.04, 0.10], suggesting that those with higher PTSD severity reported higher average levels of momentary negative thought, consistent with H1. Subdomain analyses showed a similar effect of PTSD severity for Self-Blame, Other-Blame, Negative Self-Attributions, and Negative Other- and World-Attributions (see Supplement Table 3).

Temporal Dynamics of Negative Thought (H2-H4)

Negative Thought Variability (H2) We used a Pearson’s correlation to examine the linear relationship between PTSD severity and variability (i.e., iSD) of negative thought. There was a significant positive association between PTSD and negative thought variability, r = 0.47, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.28, 0.63], consistent with H2. After adjusting for mean levels of negative thought using a partial correlation, the association between PTSD severity and variability in negative thought was reduced but still significant, r = 0.24, p = 0.030. Exploratory analyses by subdomain showed a similar pattern: PTSD severity was positively associated with iSD for all subdomains (ps =  < 0.001); however, only the relationship between the iSD of Negative Self-Attributions and PTSD severity were significant after adjusting for mean levels of negative thought (Supplement Table 4).
Negative Thought Inertia (H3) We next examined if negative thought is self-perpetuating across time and whether PTSD severity affects the strength of negative thought inertia. The following slopes-as-outcomes model was estimated with L2 PTSD severity predicting the L1 auto-correlation of negative thought across time:
Level 1:
$$ {\text{Negative Thought}}_{\left( t \right)ij} = \beta_{0j} + \beta_{1j} \left( {{\text{Negative Thought}}_{{\left( {t - 1} \right)}} } \right) + r_{ij} $$
Level 2:
$$ \begin{gathered} \beta_{0j} = \gamma_{00} + \gamma_{01} \left( {{\text{PTSD Severity}}_{j} } \right) + \mu_{0j} \hfill \\ \beta_{1j} = \gamma_{10} + \gamma_{11} \left( {{\text{PTSD Severity}}_{j} } \right) + \gamma_{12} \left( {{\text{Negative Thought}}_{j} } \right) + \mu_{1j} \hfill \\ \end{gathered} $$
At L1, the β1 parameter represents the autocorrelation of negative thought over successive observations at each person’s mean level of negative thought. This value conceptually represents inertia, or how self-perpetuating negative thought is across time. The γ10 parameter at L2 reflects the slope of the autocorrelation and the γ11 parameter represents the effect of PTSD severity on inertia. All other values are as described in the means-as-outcomes model (H1) above. Results show that negative thought inertia was significant across observations, b = 0.20, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.12, 0.28], see Table 2. Inconsistent with H3, PTSD severity did not predict negative thought inertia, p = 0.432 (p = 0.666 in unadjusted model). Exploratory analyses showed an identical pattern of findings for all negative thought subdomains. An inertia effect was present for Self-Blame, Negative Self-Attributions, and Negative Other- and World-Attributions, but not for Other-Blame. No cross-level interactions effects were significant (see Supplement Table 5).
Table 2
Cross-level interactions of PTSD severity on negative thought intensity, inertia, and reactivity in daily life
NT intensity
b
SE
p
95% CI
Fixed effects
    
  NT intercept (γ00)
5.51
0.19
 < 0.001
[5.14, 5.87]
  PTSD severity (γ01)
0.07
0.01
 < 0.001
[0.04, 0.10]
Random effects
    
  NT intercept (u0j)
4.15
0.56
 < 0.0001
[3.04, 5.26]
  Level 1 residual (rij)
2.53
0.63
 < 0.001
[1.30, 3.77]
NT inertia
b
SE
p
95% CI
Fixed effects
    
  NT intercept (γ00)
5.32
0.17
 < 0.001
[4.99, 5.66]
  NT inertia slope (γ10)
0.20
0.04
 < 0.001
[0.12, 0.28]
  PTSD severity (γ01)
0.06
0.02
 < 0.001
[0.03, 0.09]
  PTSD severity × NT inertia (γ11)
 − 0.00
0.01
0.432
[− 0.01, 0.01]
  Person-mean NT × NT inertia (γ11)
0.12
0.04
0.002
[0.04, 0.19]
Random effects
    
  NT intercept (u0j)
1.99
0.49
 < 0.001
[1.03, 2.94]
  NT inertia slope (u1j)
0.09
0.04
0.014
[0.02, 0.15]
  Level 1 residual (rij)
3.19
0.52
 < 0.001
[2.18, 4.20]
NT reactivity
b
SE
p
95% CI
Fixed effects
    
  NT intercept (γ00)
5.36
0.17
 < 0.001
[5.03, 5.70]
  NT inertia slope (γ10)
0.15
0.04
 < 0.001
[0.07, 0.22]
  NT reactivity slope (γ20)
0.54
0.08
 < 0.001
[0.38, 0.70]
  PTSD severity (γ01)
0.06
0.02
 < 0.001
[0.03, 0.09]
  PTSD severity × NT inertia (γ11)
 − 0.01
0.00
0.176
[-0.01, 0.00]
  Person-mean NT × NT inertia (γ12)
0.10
0.03
0.004
[0.03, 0.16]
  PTSD severity × NT reactivity (γ21)
0.02
0.01
0.031
[0.00, 0.04]
  Person-mean NT × NT reactivity (γ22)
0.09
0.08
0.255
[− 0.07, 0.25]
Random effects
    
  NT intercept (u0j)
2.00
0.46
 < 0.001
[1.11, 2.90]
  NT inertia slope (u1j)
0.05
0.02
0.010
[0.01, 0.08]
  NT reactivity slope (u2j)
0.28
0.12
0.016
[0.05, 0.50]
  Level 1 residual (rij)
2.41
0.35
 < 0.001
[1.73, 3.09]
PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; NT = negative thought. Model parameters of interest are bolded. NT Inertia (γ10) reflects the within-subjects influence of NT at time t on NT at time t + 1. NT Reactivity (γ20) reflects concurrent within-subjects shifts between perceived situational stress and NT. The γ11 (not sig.) and γ21 (sig.) parameters represent the cross-level interactions PTSD severity on NT Inertia and NT Reactivity, respectively. Inertia and reactivity models are adjusted for person-mean levels of negative thought.
Negative Thought Reactivity (H4) Finally, we tested reactivity by examining if perceived situational stress is associated with corresponding shifts in negative thought, and whether PTSD severity affects the strength of these shifts. We estimated the following slopes-as-outcomes models with L2 PTSD severity predicting the L1 relationship between perceived situational stress and current negative thought, adjusted for negative thought at the previous observation and person-specific mean levels of negative thought:
Level 1:
$$ \begin{aligned} {\text{Negative Thought}}_{{\left( t \right)ij}} = & \beta _{{0j}} + \beta _{{1j}} \left( {{\text{Negative Thought}}_{{\left( {t - 1} \right)}} } \right) \\ & + \beta _{{2j}} \left( {{\text{Perceived Stress}}_{{\left( t \right)}} } \right) + r_{{{\text{ij}}}} \\ \end{aligned} $$
Level 2:
$$ \begin{gathered} \beta_{0j} = \gamma_{00} + \gamma_{01} \left( {{\text{PTSD Severity}}_{j} } \right) + \mu_{0j} \hfill \\ \beta_{1j} = \gamma_{10} + \gamma_{11} \left( {{\text{PTSD Severity}}_{j} } \right) + \gamma_{12} \left( {{\text{Negative Thought}}_{j} } \right) + \mu_{1j} \hfill \\ \beta_{2j} = \gamma_{20} + \gamma_{21} \left( {{\text{PTSD Severity}}_{j} } \right) + \gamma_{22} \left( {{\text{Negative Thought}}_{j} } \right) + \mu_{2j} \hfill \\ \end{gathered} $$
At L1, the β2 parameter represents how much perceived situational stress is associated with concurrent shifts in negative thought, adjusted for negative thought at the previous observation. The γ20 parameter represents the slope of reactivity and the γ21 parameter represents the effect of PTSD severity on reactivity. All other parameters are consistent with the inertia model above. Results show that after adjusting for previous levels of negative thought and person-specific mean levels of negative thought, there was a significant effect of perceived situational stress on concurrent negative thought, b = 0.54, SE = 0.08, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.38, 0.70], see Table 2. The effect of PTSD severity was significant, b = 0.02, SE = 0.01, p = 0.031, 95% CI [0.002, 0.04] such that those with higher PTSD severity experienced greater shifts in negative thought associated with perceived stressful situations, consistent with H4 (unadjusted model p = 0.001, 95% CI [0.012, 0.046]). Exploratory analyses showed a similar pattern of findings for reactivity across subdomains. The effect of PTSD severity was significant in all unadjusted models (ps range from < 0.001 to 0.015), but analyses adjusted for person-mean levels showed that the cross-level interaction of PTSD severity was only significant for Negative Self-Attributions (p = 0.029, Supplement Table 6).

Discussion

The goals of this study were to examine the temporal dynamics of negative thought in daily life among trauma-exposed adults with varying PTSD severity. Consistent with a large body of retrospective self-report questionnaire research demonstrating associations between negative cognitions and PTSD (Brown et al., 2019; Losavio et al., 2017; Moulds et al., 2020), results showed that individuals with higher PTSD severity reported higher average levels of negative thought in daily life. Negative thought fluctuated considerably over time, however, and variability in negative thought was explained by both state (within-subjects) and dispositional (between-subjects) factors. Further, the temporal dynamics derived from these fluctuations (i.e., negative thought variability and reactivity, but not inertia) were elevated in people with higher PTSD severity. Findings largely held after adjusting for person-specific means in negative thought, suggesting that the dynamics of negative thought provide a unique window into the cognitive experience of PTSD.

Negative Thought Dynamics in PTSD

Consistent with our predictions, trauma-exposed adults reported marked fluctuation in their endorsement of negative thought on an hour-to-hour basis (Fig. 1). Variability in these fluctuations was greater among individuals with higher PTSD severity, even after adjusting for mean levels, suggesting that people with higher PTSD severity experience more frequent and intense moments of negative thought compared to individuals with less severe PTSD. This finding is consistent with current symptoms descriptions in DSM-5-TR (APA, 2022) and with research showing that variability in other cognitive processes (i.e., rumination) is associated with psychopathology (Bean & Ciesla, 2024; Kiekens et al., 2024). At the same time, findings suggest that despite higher mean levels of negative thought, people with higher PTSD severity have many moments when they are not experiencing negative thought like blame or negative attributions. Well-timed cognitively focused interventions could potentially leverage moments when patients are not blaming or thinking negatively about themselves, others, or the world to identify more adaptive patterns of thinking.
That people with higher PTSD severity display heightened negative thought reactivity provides some preliminary context for how negative thought unfolds in daily life. Extensive theory on cognitive vulnerability—the idea that negative thoughts emerge in stressful situations—has shown that cognitive reactivity characterizes people at risk for depression and precipitates its onset and recurrence (Beck, 1967). Here, we found that people with higher PTSD severity reported heightened negative thought during moments of perceived situational stress, even after adjusting for mean levels of negative thought, suggesting that cognitive reactivity is a distinct feature of PTSD. Findings align with cognitive theory of PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, 2000) and idiographic network analysis research showing that negative thought is a prominent maintenance factor for PTSD across people (Bridges-Curry, 2025; Reeves & Fisher, 2020). That is, heightened cognitive reactivity could reflect misperceptions of ongoing threat in PTSD and trouble processing disconfirming information. In turn, negative thoughts could become more easily reinforced and contribute to symptom chronicity. Other studies examining the temporal structure of PTSD have found that hyperarousal symptoms (e.g., startle response) are most central to maintaining PTSD, however (Greene, 2021), suggesting that there is heterogeneity across people in how PTSD is maintained.
Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not find that negative thought inertia varied based on PTSD severity. This stands in contrast to recent work finding that ruminative inertia is elevated in individuals with depression and anxiety (Bean & Ciesla, 2024; Bean et al., 2020, 2021), and extensive research documenting a robust association between rumination and PTSD (Moulds et al., 2020). We did find that negative thought was inert on average, suggesting that negative thought generally persists for trauma-exposed adults over time. This could be taken to mean that negative thought inertia is not a unique feature of PTSD severity, but rather that once negative thought begins, it is generally difficult for most people to dismiss. Further, although inertia may not necessarily be stronger in those with more severe PTSD, it may be more problematic because negative thought occurs more frequently in this population.
In sum, findings suggest that heightened PTSD severity is characterized by negative thought patterns that are less stable and more readily activated by perceived stress, but not more resistant to change over time. Conceptually, this pattern suggests that individuals with more severe PTSD experience larger and more extreme activation of negative posttraumatic thought, particularly during stressful moments, with negative thought exhibiting comparable return to baseline regardless of PTSD severity. Our findings have implications for future studies on the cognitive dynamics of PTSD and the development and adaptation of interventions for PTSD.

Measurement Considerations

That research on negative cognition in PTSD is over-represented by static, between-subjects, and cross-sectional assessments raises questions about how these processes unfold within people over time. Our results show that when negative thought is repeatedly probed, trauma-exposed individuals vary considerably in how much they espouse negative thought at different moments in time. The tendency for negative thought to vary occurs on relatively short timescales, shown here on an hour-to-hour basis. This variability is driven both by state and dispositional factors and isolating within-subjects effects provided unique information over and above person-specific averages. The continued adoption of ambulatory methods that repeatedly track people over time and statistical approaches that disaggregate within- and between-subjects processes will help advance our understanding of cognitive processes involved in PTSD. Our findings produced some recommendations for this future research.
Our hypotheses were informed by existing research on the cognitive dynamics of depression. Research in this area has often adopted daily measures of rumination adapted from the Ruminative Responses Subscale of the Response Styles Questionnaire (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). These questions tap the tendency to ruminate, including thoughts about causes, symptoms, and reactions to negative mood. By contrast, our assessment of negative thought focused on specific content domains prevalent in PTSD (blame, negative attributions; Foa et al., 1999). Despite the conceptual overlap between rumination and negative thought, they are distinct in operationalization and measurement, which may add context to our findings. Rumination is a process that exacerbates and prolongs depression throughout a variety of mechanisms, one of which is by enhancing negative thought (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Rumination might “cut across” different types of negative thought to support a proclivity toward maladaptive thinking, particularly among individuals inclined towards negative mood. Assessments of rumination, particularly over longer intervals (e.g., day-to-day; Bean et al., 2020, 2021), could better reflect the perseverative process thought to keep individuals “cognitively stuck” (Koval et al., 2012). By contrast, our dense sampling schedule showed that momentary endorsement of negative thought content was infrequent, especially for Self-Blame and Other-Blame, which were only present on 5.9% and 7.1% of occasions, respectively. The low frequency of negative thought over this period may have contributed to difficulty detecting the cognitive dynamics of interest. Likewise, this sampling frame may have also introduced challenges for model estimation. Although we took steps to address low frequency of endorsement in extreme cases (e.g., dichotomizing the blame variables and using logistic regression), findings suggest that the cognitive dynamics of negative thought such as variability and inertia might be better captured over less frequent intervals (e.g., multi-hour, day-to-day). Future research measuring both rumination and negative thought in daily life will help clarify the relationship between these cognitive processes.
Finally, the negative thought dynamics examined here have transdiagnostic implications. Findings join a growing body of research showing that variability, inertia, and reactivity in cognitive processes are associated with depression, anxiety, and NSSI (Bean & Ciesla, 2024; Bean et al., 2020, 2021; Cole et al., 2021; Kiekens et al., 2024). Though the focus of our study was on trauma-exposed adults varying in PTSD severity, most people with PTSD reported at least one current comorbid disorder, consistent with national incidence data (96.5% PTSD comorbidity rate with at least one other disorder; Gadermann et al., 2012). Notably, results held after adjusting for MDD comorbidity, specifically, and psychiatric comorbidity, generally. These findings raise two possibilities. First, whereas previous research on cognitive dynamics has largely focused on rumination in depressed samples, the dynamics of negative posttraumatic thought assessed here could be specific to PTSD. Negative cognitive processes such as negative thought are central cognitive features of PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, 2000). Coupled with the high comorbidity shared with other disorders observed following trauma exposure, it is possible that PTSD is the primary driver of these dynamics in trauma exposed individuals. In other words, the cognitive dynamics of negative thought could be due to shared comorbidity. Second, these cognitive dynamics may be features of and share similar vulnerabilities for elevated scores on broader dimensions that cut across PTSD and other psychiatric disorders (e.g., fear, distress), consistent with transdiagnostic conceptualizations of psychopathology (Kotov et al., 2021). A transdiagnostic perspective would help explain emerging research documenting dynamic features of cognition that characterize depression, anxiety, NSSI, and now PTSD. Unfortunately, our tests for covariation used a categorical measure of psychiatric disorder diagnostic comorbidity that may be underpowered compared to the dimensional measure of PTSD, leaving us unable to rule out method effects. Future work in trauma exposed samples would benefit from including transdiagnostic samples and assessment tools to better understand if the cognitive dynamics of negative posttraumatic thought serve as a transdiagnostic or disorder-specific marker of PTSD. Additionally, research should also consider longitudinal designs that can speak to whether cognitive dynamics serve as markers for psychopathology or confer risk for its development and maintenance following trauma exposure.

Clinical Implications

Findings suggest that standard delivery of cognitive interventions for negative thought (e.g., weekly outpatient treatment with little-to-no between-session support; Schleider et al., 2021) might be suboptimal for the timescale on which it varies in daily life. Alternative formats that increase the frequency of cognitive interventions could better match how negative thoughts fluctuate in daily life. Optimizing the timing of intervention delivery could help patients gain insight into their own cognitive processes (e.g., when and in what contexts negative thought typically occurs) and learn to use therapy skills when needed most. Massed delivery formats (e.g., massed CPT), wherein interventions are delivered at an accelerated pace of multiple times per day over consecutive days, have been shown to reduce PTSD symptoms with large effects comparable to standard delivery methods and hold promise for improving treatment access, efficiency, and retention (Galovski et al., 2022; Held et al., 2022). Integrating ambulatory assessments into massed treatment opens the door for intriguing questions, such as whether changes in the temporal dynamics of negative thought track or precede symptom improvement. For example, negative thoughts may become less variable, “sticky,” and reactive as patients learn to skillfully challenge them in therapy. In addition to more frequent therapist intervention, just-in-time adaptive interventions (JITAI) also hold promise in reducing negative thought by providing tailored, technology-delivered support in the moment as needed (Nahum-Shani et al., 2018). JITAIs may be self-initiated by the user during high-stress moments or automatically triggered via passive sensing (e.g., ambulatory physiological assessment; Wisco et al., 2024 under review) to promote skills use in real-time. JITAIs may be used in conjunction with standard psychotherapy or as stand-alone interventions capable of increasing treatment efficacy and global accessibility to treatment, and several mobile health applications already exist to assist in the treatment of PTSD (Rodriguez-Paras et al., 2017).
Finally, subdomain analyses highlight the importance of examining and treating specific negative thought types. Though the temporal dynamics identified here were generally consistent across subdomains, subdomains differed in in their frequency of endorsement. Endorsement of Negative Self-Attributions and Negative Other- and World-Attributions were most common (23.5% and 19.7% of observations, respectively), whereas Self-Blame and Other-Blame were endorsed infrequently. Cognitive dynamics for Negative Self-Attributions were most robust, as this was the only subdomain to survive adjustment for person-mean levels of negative thought for variability and reactivity. Findings are particularly relevant for treatment studies seeking to identify within-person mechanisms of change (Alpert et al., 2023), given discrepant findings regarding which subdomains are altered in treatment (Dillon et al., 2020; Holliday et al., 2018; Schumm et al., 2015). Findings suggest that targeting Negative Self-Attributions may be particularly important given its frequency and role in demarcating PTSD severity. However, given potential person-to-person differences in the subdomains of negative thought endorsed, personalized approaches that identify which thoughts are most relevant for a given person and how these thoughts change in response to trauma-focused treatment also hold great value (Brown et al., 2019).

Limitations

There are a few limitations of our study that can inform future work. First, though the racial/ethnic diversity of this sample is a strength, this was largely a community sample of young females. Future work will need to examine if results generalize to other sociodemographic groups and settings where the prevalence and domains of negative thoughts might differ (e.g., in treatment seeking individuals or individuals with primary diagnoses other than PTSD). Second, in terms of design, lack of personalization around our assessment interval (~ 9 am to 11:30 pm) precludes capturing the experiences of those who wake up and go to bed either earlier or later. Future designs might customize the sampling period to each participant’s schedule to avoid this issue, in addition to employing a sampling schedule that is better suited to capture negative thought in daily life (i.e., a longer sampling period within less frequent within-day assessment).
Third, our measures of self-reported situational stress and negative thoughts left us unable to parse the effects of objective stress exposure from an individual’s subjective appraisals. In other words, perceptions of situational stress could be influenced by negative thought. The confounding of stressful stimuli and response is prominent in the stress literature. One possible solution follows the use of gold-standard life stress interviews (e.g., Hammen et al., 1987), which use multiple independent raters blind to participant diagnoses and responses to rate episodic life stress. Interview methods yield a measure of inter-rater reliability while capturing idiosyncratic stress appraisals of each rater. Application of this method to ambulatory designs is limited by inefficiency and impracticality, though some have proposed feasible solutions for integration (Harkness & Monroe, 2016). Another solution consistent with recent recommendations (Wright et al., 2019) is to incorporate passive sensing methods to objectively monitor and capture changes in one’s environment (e.g., GPS location, voice activity) or stress response (e.g., accelerometer or physiological sensing, text messages). Developing plausible and scalable measures of stress for ambulatory methods that enable researchers to parse distinct components of stressors and stress responses will be an important goal for future research.
Finally, another limitation is the contemporaneous method we used to assess negative thought reactivity. We operationalized reactivity as the within-subjects associations between concurrent perceived situational stress and negative thought, adjusted for negative thought in the previous survey. This precluded us from establishing a precise temporal relationship between perceived stress and negative thought. At minimum, we can be confident that results establish hour-to-hour covariation between perceived situational stress and negative thought, and that this association exists beyond the tendency for negative thought to persist from one moment to the next. Ambulatory designs examining the temporal features of negative thought will need to carefully measure contextual characteristics to clarify their influence on negative thought over time.

Conclusion

Cognitive theories posit that negative posttraumatic thought plays an important role in the development, maintenance, and treatment of PTSD. Extant research on negative thought in PTSD is over-represented by static, between-subjects, and cross-sectional assessments using retrospective self-report. Using ecological momentary assessment, we show that negative thought unfolds dynamically within people over short timescales. The temporal dynamics of negative thought demonstrated incremental value over mean levels in distinguishing people based on PTSD severity, suggesting that they may serve as potential markers of PTSD. Findings provide insights into the conceptualization of negative thought and its role in the experience and treatment of PTSD.

Acknowledgements

We thank our research staff, especially Casey May, Faith Nomamiukor, Allison Campbell, Sophia Priest, Madison Ellis, and Amanda King, for their work on this project. We are grateful to the participants whose collaboration made this project possible.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no competing interests.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

BSL GOP_opleiding GZ-psycholoog

Bijlagen

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Voetnoten
1
Previous work shows that variability can be quantified using within-person variance and mean-square of successive difference (MSSD). We report the iSD as our measure of variability to facilitate interpretation, and because it is a pure estimate of overall dispersion of scores over time. The MSSD, by comparison, includes a temporal component in its calculation (Jahng et al., 2008). Further, these measures are often redundant and highly-correlated (Dejonckheere et al., 2019).
 
2
Participants completed 52.9% (SD = 18.4) of all surveys, which includes surveys not administered due to late start times in the morning or bedtimes in the evenings. No group differences in compliance emerged, t(78) = 0.605, p = 0.547.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Alpert, E., Tabke, C. S., Cole, T. A., Lee, D. J., & Sloan, D. M. (2023). A systematic review of literature examining mediators and mechanisms of change in empirically supported treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology Review,103, 102300.PubMedCrossRef Alpert, E., Tabke, C. S., Cole, T. A., Lee, D. J., & Sloan, D. M. (2023). A systematic review of literature examining mediators and mechanisms of change in empirically supported treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology Review,103, 102300.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
go back to reference Arbuckle, J. L. (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In G. A. Marcoulides & R. E. Schumacker (Eds.), Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp. 243–277). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Arbuckle, J. L. (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In G. A. Marcoulides & R. E. Schumacker (Eds.), Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp. 243–277). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
go back to reference Bean, C. A., & Ciesla, J. A. (2024). Ruminative variability predicts increases in depression and social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research,48(3), 511–525.PubMedCrossRef Bean, C. A., & Ciesla, J. A. (2024). Ruminative variability predicts increases in depression and social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research,48(3), 511–525.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bean, C. A., Heggeness, L. F., & Ciesla, J. A. (2021). Ruminative inertia, emotion regulation, and depression: A daily-diary study. Behavior Therapy,52(6), 1477–1488.PubMedCrossRef Bean, C. A., Heggeness, L. F., & Ciesla, J. A. (2021). Ruminative inertia, emotion regulation, and depression: A daily-diary study. Behavior Therapy,52(6), 1477–1488.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bean, C. A., Heggeness, L. F., Kalmbach, D. A., & Ciesla, J. A. (2020). Ruminative inertia and its association with current severity and lifetime course of depression. Clinical Psychological Science,8(6), 1007–1016.CrossRef Bean, C. A., Heggeness, L. F., Kalmbach, D. A., & Ciesla, J. A. (2020). Ruminative inertia and its association with current severity and lifetime course of depression. Clinical Psychological Science,8(6), 1007–1016.CrossRef
go back to reference Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. University of Pennsylvania Press. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. University of Pennsylvania Press.
go back to reference Blanke, E. S., Neubauer, A. B., Houben, M., Erbas, Y., & Brose, A. (2022). Why do my thoughts feel so bad? Getting at the reciprocal effects of rumination and negative affect using dynamic structural equation modeling. Emotion,22(8), 1773.PubMedCrossRef Blanke, E. S., Neubauer, A. B., Houben, M., Erbas, Y., & Brose, A. (2022). Why do my thoughts feel so bad? Getting at the reciprocal effects of rumination and negative affect using dynamic structural equation modeling. Emotion,22(8), 1773.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bovin, M. J., Marx, B. P., Weathers, F. W., Gallagher, M. W., Rodriguez, P., Schnurr, P. P., & Keane, T. M. (2016). Psychometric properties of the PTSD checklist for diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-fifth edition (PCL-5) in veterans. Psychological Assessment,28(11), 1379–1391.PubMedCrossRef Bovin, M. J., Marx, B. P., Weathers, F. W., Gallagher, M. W., Rodriguez, P., Schnurr, P. P., & Keane, T. M. (2016). Psychometric properties of the PTSD checklist for diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-fifth edition (PCL-5) in veterans. Psychological Assessment,28(11), 1379–1391.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bridges-Curry, Z. (2025). An idiographic network approach to posttraumatic stress disorder: Symptom heterogeneity and implications for treatment. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 1–11. Bridges-Curry, Z. (2025). An idiographic network approach to posttraumatic stress disorder: Symptom heterogeneity and implications for treatment. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 1–11.
go back to reference Brown, L. A., Belli, G. M., Asnaani, A., & Foa, E. B. (2019). A review of the role of negative cognitions about oneself, others, and the world in the treatment of PTSD. Cognitive Therapy and Research,43, 143–173.CrossRef Brown, L. A., Belli, G. M., Asnaani, A., & Foa, E. B. (2019). A review of the role of negative cognitions about oneself, others, and the world in the treatment of PTSD. Cognitive Therapy and Research,43, 143–173.CrossRef
go back to reference Cole, D. A., Gabruk, M. E., Nestor, B. A., Liu, Q., Lovette, A. J., Zhang, Y., & Mick, C. R. (2021). Between-versus within-person emotional and cognitive reactivity in relation to depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders,295, 479–487.PubMedCrossRef Cole, D. A., Gabruk, M. E., Nestor, B. A., Liu, Q., Lovette, A. J., Zhang, Y., & Mick, C. R. (2021). Between-versus within-person emotional and cognitive reactivity in relation to depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders,295, 479–487.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cusack, K., Jonas, D. E., Forneris, C. A., Wines, C., Sonis, J., Middleton, J. C., Feltner, C., Brownley, K. A., Olmsted, K. R., Greenblatt, A., Weil, A., & Gaynes, B. N. (2016). Psychological treatments for adults with posttraumatic stress disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review,43, 128–141.PubMedCrossRef Cusack, K., Jonas, D. E., Forneris, C. A., Wines, C., Sonis, J., Middleton, J. C., Feltner, C., Brownley, K. A., Olmsted, K. R., Greenblatt, A., Weil, A., & Gaynes, B. N. (2016). Psychological treatments for adults with posttraumatic stress disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review,43, 128–141.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dejonckheere, E., Mestdagh, M., Houben, M., Rutten, I., Sels, L., Kuppens, P., & Tuerlinckx, F. (2019). Complex affect dynamics add limited information to the prediction of psychological wellbeing. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 478–491. Dejonckheere, E., Mestdagh, M., Houben, M., Rutten, I., Sels, L., Kuppens, P., & Tuerlinckx, F. (2019). Complex affect dynamics add limited information to the prediction of psychological wellbeing. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 478–491.
go back to reference Dillon, K. H., Hale, W. J., LoSavio, S. T., Wachen, J. S., Pruiksma, K. E., Yarvis, J. S., Mintz, J., Litz, B. T., Peterson, A. L., Resick, P. A., STRONG STAR Consortium. (2020). Weekly changes in blame and PTSD among active-duty military personnel receiving cognitive processing therapy. Behavior Therapy,51(3), 386–400.PubMedCrossRef Dillon, K. H., Hale, W. J., LoSavio, S. T., Wachen, J. S., Pruiksma, K. E., Yarvis, J. S., Mintz, J., Litz, B. T., Peterson, A. L., Resick, P. A., STRONG STAR Consortium. (2020). Weekly changes in blame and PTSD among active-duty military personnel receiving cognitive processing therapy. Behavior Therapy,51(3), 386–400.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research & Therapy,38(4), 319–345.CrossRef Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research & Therapy,38(4), 319–345.CrossRef
go back to reference Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Hackmann, A., McManus, F., & Fennell, M. (2005). Cognitive therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder: Development and evaluation. Behaviour Research & Therapy,43(4), 413–431.CrossRef Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Hackmann, A., McManus, F., & Fennell, M. (2005). Cognitive therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder: Development and evaluation. Behaviour Research & Therapy,43(4), 413–431.CrossRef
go back to reference First, M. B., Williams, J. B. W., Karg, R. S., & Spitzer, R. L. (2015). Structured clinical interview for DSM-5-research version (SCID-5-RV, version 1.0.0). American Psychiatric Association. First, M. B., Williams, J. B. W., Karg, R. S., & Spitzer, R. L. (2015). Structured clinical interview for DSM-5-research version (SCID-5-RV, version 1.0.0). American Psychiatric Association.
go back to reference Foa, E. B., Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Tolin, D. F., & Orsillo, S. M. (1999). The posttraumatic cognitions inventory (PTCI): Development and validation. Psychological Assessment,11(3), 303.CrossRef Foa, E. B., Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Tolin, D. F., & Orsillo, S. M. (1999). The posttraumatic cognitions inventory (PTCI): Development and validation. Psychological Assessment,11(3), 303.CrossRef
go back to reference Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., & Cahill, S. P. (2006). Emotional processing theory: An update. In B.O. Rothbaum (Ed.), Pathological anxiety: Emotional processing in etiology and treatment (pp. 3–24). Guilford Press. Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., & Cahill, S. P. (2006). Emotional processing theory: An update. In B.O. Rothbaum (Ed.), Pathological anxiety: Emotional processing in etiology and treatment (pp. 3–24). Guilford Press.
go back to reference Gadermann, A. M., Alonso, J., Vilagut, G., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2012). Comorbidity and disease burden in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Depression and Anxiety,29(9), 797–806.PubMedCrossRef Gadermann, A. M., Alonso, J., Vilagut, G., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2012). Comorbidity and disease burden in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Depression and Anxiety,29(9), 797–806.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Galovski, T. E., Werner, K. B., Weaver, T. L., Morris, K. L., Dondanville, K. A., Nanney, J., Wamser-Nanney, R., McGlinchey, G., Fortier, C. B., & Iverson, K. M. (2022). Massed cognitive processing therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in women survivors of intimate partner violence. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy,14(5), 769–779.PubMedCrossRef Galovski, T. E., Werner, K. B., Weaver, T. L., Morris, K. L., Dondanville, K. A., Nanney, J., Wamser-Nanney, R., McGlinchey, G., Fortier, C. B., & Iverson, K. M. (2022). Massed cognitive processing therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in women survivors of intimate partner violence. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy,14(5), 769–779.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Greene, T. (2021). The dynamic interactions of PTSD symptoms in daily life. European Journal of Psychotraumatology,12(sup1), 1866413.PubMedCentralCrossRef Greene, T. (2021). The dynamic interactions of PTSD symptoms in daily life. European Journal of Psychotraumatology,12(sup1), 1866413.PubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Hamaker, E. L., & Wichers, M. (2017). No time like the present: Discovering the hidden dynamics in intensive longitudinal data. Current Directions in Psychological Science,26(1), 10–15.CrossRef Hamaker, E. L., & Wichers, M. (2017). No time like the present: Discovering the hidden dynamics in intensive longitudinal data. Current Directions in Psychological Science,26(1), 10–15.CrossRef
go back to reference Hammen, C., Adrian, C., Gordon, D., Burge, D., Jaenicke, C., & Hiroto, D. (1987). Children of depressed mothers: Maternal strain and symptom predictors of dysfunction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,96, 190–198.PubMedCrossRef Hammen, C., Adrian, C., Gordon, D., Burge, D., Jaenicke, C., & Hiroto, D. (1987). Children of depressed mothers: Maternal strain and symptom predictors of dysfunction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,96, 190–198.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Harkness, K. L., & Monroe, S. M. (2016). The assessment and measurement of adult life stress: Basic premises, operational principles, and design requirements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,125(5), 727.PubMedCrossRef Harkness, K. L., & Monroe, S. M. (2016). The assessment and measurement of adult life stress: Basic premises, operational principles, and design requirements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,125(5), 727.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Held, P., Kovacevic, M., Petrey, K., Meade, E. A., Pridgen, S., Montes, M., Werner, B., Miller, M. L., Smith, D. L., Kaysen, D., & Karnik, N. S. (2022). Treating posttraumatic stress disorder at home in a single week using 1-week virtual massed cognitive processing therapy. Journal of Traumatic Stress,35(4), 1215–1225.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Held, P., Kovacevic, M., Petrey, K., Meade, E. A., Pridgen, S., Montes, M., Werner, B., Miller, M. L., Smith, D. L., Kaysen, D., & Karnik, N. S. (2022). Treating posttraumatic stress disorder at home in a single week using 1-week virtual massed cognitive processing therapy. Journal of Traumatic Stress,35(4), 1215–1225.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Herzog, S., Fogle, B. M., Harpaz-Rotem, I., Tsai, J., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2020). Dissociative symptoms in a nationally representative sample of trauma-exposed US military veterans: Prevalence, comorbidities, and suicidality. Journal of Affective Disorders,272, 138–145.PubMedCrossRef Herzog, S., Fogle, B. M., Harpaz-Rotem, I., Tsai, J., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2020). Dissociative symptoms in a nationally representative sample of trauma-exposed US military veterans: Prevalence, comorbidities, and suicidality. Journal of Affective Disorders,272, 138–145.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Holliday, R., Holder, N., & Surís, A. (2018). Reductions in self-blame cognitions predict PTSD improvements with cognitive processing therapy for military sexual trauma-related PTSD. Psychiatry Research,263, 181–184.PubMedCrossRef Holliday, R., Holder, N., & Surís, A. (2018). Reductions in self-blame cognitions predict PTSD improvements with cognitive processing therapy for military sexual trauma-related PTSD. Psychiatry Research,263, 181–184.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Houben, M., Van Den Noortgate, W., & Kuppens, P. (2015). The relation between short-term emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,141(4), 901.PubMedCrossRef Houben, M., Van Den Noortgate, W., & Kuppens, P. (2015). The relation between short-term emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,141(4), 901.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jahng, S., Wood, P. K., & Trull, T. J. (2008). Analysis of affective instability in ecological momentary assessment: Indices using successive difference and group comparison via multilevel modeling. Psychological Methods, 13(4), 354. Jahng, S., Wood, P. K., & Trull, T. J. (2008). Analysis of affective instability in ecological momentary assessment: Indices using successive difference and group comparison via multilevel modeling. Psychological Methods, 13(4), 354.
go back to reference Kiekens, G., Hasking, P., Nock, M. K., Kleiman, E., Kirtley, O. J., Houben, M., Boyes, M., Bruffaerts, R., Myin-Germeys, I., & Claes, L. (2024). A comparison of affective-cognitive states in daily life between emerging adults with and without past-year nonsuicidal self-injury. Behavior Therapy,55(3), 469–484.PubMedCrossRef Kiekens, G., Hasking, P., Nock, M. K., Kleiman, E., Kirtley, O. J., Houben, M., Boyes, M., Bruffaerts, R., Myin-Germeys, I., & Claes, L. (2024). A comparison of affective-cognitive states in daily life between emerging adults with and without past-year nonsuicidal self-injury. Behavior Therapy,55(3), 469–484.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Klein, A. B., Schnurr, P. P., Bovin, M. J., Friedman, M. J., Keane, T. M., & Marx, B. P. (2024). An empirical investigation of definitions of subthreshold posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress,37(1), 113–125.PubMedCrossRef Klein, A. B., Schnurr, P. P., Bovin, M. J., Friedman, M. J., Keane, T. M., & Marx, B. P. (2024). An empirical investigation of definitions of subthreshold posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress,37(1), 113–125.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Cicero, D. C., Conway, C. C., DeYoung, C. G., & Wright, A. G. (2021). The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): A quantitative nosology based on consensus of evidence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,17(1), 83–108.PubMedCrossRef Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Cicero, D. C., Conway, C. C., DeYoung, C. G., & Wright, A. G. (2021). The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): A quantitative nosology based on consensus of evidence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,17(1), 83–108.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koval, P., Kuppens, P., Allen, N. B., & Sheeber, L. (2012). Getting stuck in depression: The roles of rumination and emotional inertia. Cognition & Emotion,26(8), 1412–1427.CrossRef Koval, P., Kuppens, P., Allen, N. B., & Sheeber, L. (2012). Getting stuck in depression: The roles of rumination and emotional inertia. Cognition & Emotion,26(8), 1412–1427.CrossRef
go back to reference Kumpula, M. J., Pentel, K. Z., Foa, E. B., LeBlanc, N. J., Bui, E., McSweeney, L. B., Knowles, K., Bosley, H., Simon, N. M., & Rauch, S. A. (2017). Temporal sequencing of change in posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptom reduction during prolonged exposure therapy. Behavior Therapy, 48(2), 156–165. Kumpula, M. J., Pentel, K. Z., Foa, E. B., LeBlanc, N. J., Bui, E., McSweeney, L. B., Knowles, K., Bosley, H., Simon, N. M., & Rauch, S. A. (2017). Temporal sequencing of change in posttraumatic cognitions and PTSD symptom reduction during prolonged exposure therapy. Behavior Therapy, 48(2), 156–165.
go back to reference Lee, D. J., Lee, L. O., Bovin, M. J., Moshier, S. J., Dutra, S. J., Kleiman, S. E., Rosen, R. C., Vasterling, J. J., Keane, T. M., & Marx, B. P. (2020). The 20-year course of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms among veterans. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,129(6), 658.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lee, D. J., Lee, L. O., Bovin, M. J., Moshier, S. J., Dutra, S. J., Kleiman, S. E., Rosen, R. C., Vasterling, J. J., Keane, T. M., & Marx, B. P. (2020). The 20-year course of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms among veterans. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,129(6), 658.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference LoSavio, S. T., Dillon, K. H., & Resick, P. A. (2017). Cognitive factors in the development, maintenance, and treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder. Current Opinion in Psychology,14, 18–22.PubMedCrossRef LoSavio, S. T., Dillon, K. H., & Resick, P. A. (2017). Cognitive factors in the development, maintenance, and treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder. Current Opinion in Psychology,14, 18–22.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Marmar, C. R., Schlenger, W., Henn-Haase, C., Qian, M., Purchia, E., Li, M., Li, M., Corry, N., Williams, C. S., Ho, C., Horesh, D., Karstoft, K., Shalev, A., & Kulka, R. A. (2015). Course of posttraumatic stress disorder 40 years after the Vietnam war: Findings from the National Vietnam Veterans Longitudinal Study. JAMA Psychiatry,72, 875–881.PubMedCrossRef Marmar, C. R., Schlenger, W., Henn-Haase, C., Qian, M., Purchia, E., Li, M., Li, M., Corry, N., Williams, C. S., Ho, C., Horesh, D., Karstoft, K., Shalev, A., & Kulka, R. A. (2015). Course of posttraumatic stress disorder 40 years after the Vietnam war: Findings from the National Vietnam Veterans Longitudinal Study. JAMA Psychiatry,72, 875–881.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Miller, T. (2004). The SIPS-screen: A brief self-report screen to detect the schizophrenia prodrome. Schizophrenia Research,78(Suppl 1), 78. Miller, T. (2004). The SIPS-screen: A brief self-report screen to detect the schizophrenia prodrome. Schizophrenia Research,78(Suppl 1), 78.
go back to reference Moulds, M. L., Bisby, M. A., Wild, J., & Bryant, R. A. (2020). Rumination in posttraumatic stress disorder: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review,82, 101910.PubMedCrossRef Moulds, M. L., Bisby, M. A., Wild, J., & Bryant, R. A. (2020). Rumination in posttraumatic stress disorder: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review,82, 101910.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nahum-Shani, I., Smith, S. N., Spring, B. J., Collins, L. M., Witkiewitz, K., Tewari, A., & Murphy, S. A. (2018). Just-in-time adaptive interventions (JITAIs) in mobile health: Key components and design principles for ongoing health behavior support. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,52(6), 446–462.PubMedCrossRef Nahum-Shani, I., Smith, S. N., Spring, B. J., Collins, L. M., Witkiewitz, K., Tewari, A., & Murphy, S. A. (2018). Just-in-time adaptive interventions (JITAIs) in mobile health: Key components and design principles for ongoing health behavior support. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,52(6), 446–462.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nezlek, J. B. (2012). Multilevel modeling analyses of diary-style data. In M. R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.), Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp. 357–383). Guilford Press. Nezlek, J. B. (2012). Multilevel modeling analyses of diary-style data. In M. R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.), Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp. 357–383). Guilford Press.
go back to reference Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science,3(5), 400–424.PubMedCrossRef Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science,3(5), 400–424.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pugach, C. P., Starr, L. R., Silvia, P. J., & Wisco, B. E. (2023). Negative emotion differentiation in trauma-exposed community members: Associations with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in daily life. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 132(8), 1007. Pugach, C. P., Starr, L. R., Silvia, P. J., & Wisco, B. E. (2023). Negative emotion differentiation in trauma-exposed community members: Associations with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in daily life. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 132(8), 1007.
go back to reference Reeves, J. W., & Fisher, A. J. (2020). An examination of idiographic networks of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Journal of Traumatic Stress,33(1), 84–95.PubMedCrossRef Reeves, J. W., & Fisher, A. J. (2020). An examination of idiographic networks of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Journal of Traumatic Stress,33(1), 84–95.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reitsema, A. M., Jeronimus, B. F., van Dijk, M., & de Jonge, P. (2022). Emotion dynamics in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic and descriptive review. Emotion,22(2), 374.PubMedCrossRef Reitsema, A. M., Jeronimus, B. F., van Dijk, M., & de Jonge, P. (2022). Emotion dynamics in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic and descriptive review. Emotion,22(2), 374.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Resick, P. A., Monson, C. M., & Chard, K. M. (2017). Cognitive processing therapy for PTSD: A comprehensive manual. Guilford Press. Resick, P. A., Monson, C. M., & Chard, K. M. (2017). Cognitive processing therapy for PTSD: A comprehensive manual. Guilford Press.
go back to reference Resick, P. A., & Schnicke, M. K. (1992). Cognitive processing therapy for sexual assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,60(5), 748–756.PubMedCrossRef Resick, P. A., & Schnicke, M. K. (1992). Cognitive processing therapy for sexual assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,60(5), 748–756.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rodriguez-Paras, C., Tippey, K., Brown, E., Sasangohar, F., Creech, S., Kum, H., Lawley, M., & Benzer, J. K. (2017). Posttraumatic stress disorder and mental health: App investigation and scoping literature review. JMIR mhealth and uhealth, 5(10), e56. Rodriguez-Paras, C., Tippey, K., Brown, E., Sasangohar, F., Creech, S., Kum, H., Lawley, M., & Benzer, J. K. (2017). Posttraumatic stress disorder and mental health: App investigation and scoping literature review. JMIR mhealth and uhealth, 5(10), e56.
go back to reference Scher, C. D., Ingram, R. E., & Segal, Z. V. (2005). Cognitive reactivity and vulnerability: Empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive diatheses in unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review,25(4), 487–510.PubMedCrossRef Scher, C. D., Ingram, R. E., & Segal, Z. V. (2005). Cognitive reactivity and vulnerability: Empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive diatheses in unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review,25(4), 487–510.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schleider, J. L., Dobias, M. L., Mullarkey, M. C., & Ollendick, T. (2021). Retiring, rethinking, and reconstructing the norm of once-weekly psychotherapy. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research,48, 4–8.PubMedCrossRef Schleider, J. L., Dobias, M. L., Mullarkey, M. C., & Ollendick, T. (2021). Retiring, rethinking, and reconstructing the norm of once-weekly psychotherapy. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research,48, 4–8.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schumm, J. A., Dickstein, B. D., Walter, K. H., Owens, G. P., & Chard, K. M. (2015). Changes in posttraumatic cognitions predict changes in posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms during cognitive processing therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,83(6), 1161–1166.PubMedCrossRef Schumm, J. A., Dickstein, B. D., Walter, K. H., Owens, G. P., & Chard, K. M. (2015). Changes in posttraumatic cognitions predict changes in posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms during cognitive processing therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,83(6), 1161–1166.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shankman, S. A., Funkhouser, C. J., Klein, D. N., Davila, J., Lerner, D., & Hee, D. (2018). Reliability and validity of severity dimensions of psychopathology assessed using the structured clinical interview for DSM-5 (SCID). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research,27(1), e1590.PubMedCrossRef Shankman, S. A., Funkhouser, C. J., Klein, D. N., Davila, J., Lerner, D., & Hee, D. (2018). Reliability and validity of severity dimensions of psychopathology assessed using the structured clinical interview for DSM-5 (SCID). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research,27(1), e1590.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Song, J., Howe, E., Oltmanns, J. R., & Fisher, A. J. (2023). Examining the concurrent and predictive validity of single items in ecological momentary assessments. Assessment,30(5), 1662–1671.PubMedCrossRef Song, J., Howe, E., Oltmanns, J. R., & Fisher, A. J. (2023). Examining the concurrent and predictive validity of single items in ecological momentary assessments. Assessment,30(5), 1662–1671.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, L. P., & Maxwell, S. E. (2015). On disaggregating between-person and within-person effects with longitudinal data using multilevel models. Psychological Methods,20(1), 63.PubMedCrossRef Wang, L. P., & Maxwell, S. E. (2015). On disaggregating between-person and within-person effects with longitudinal data using multilevel models. Psychological Methods,20(1), 63.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Weathers, F. W., Blake, D. D., Schnurr, P. P., Kaloupek, D. G., Marx, B. P., & Keane, T. M. (2013a). The clinician-administered PTSD scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5). Interview available from the National Center for PTSD at https://www.ptsd.va.gov Weathers, F. W., Blake, D. D., Schnurr, P. P., Kaloupek, D. G., Marx, B. P., & Keane, T. M. (2013a). The clinician-administered PTSD scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5). Interview available from the National Center for PTSD at https://​www.​ptsd.​va.​gov
go back to reference Wisco, B. E., Pugach, C. P., May, C. L., & Silvia, P. J. (2024). Ambulatory physiological assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder: Integrating passive sensing with experience sampling to measure trauma reactivity [Manuscript under review]. University of North Carolina at Greensboro. Wisco, B. E., Pugach, C. P., May, C. L., & Silvia, P. J. (2024). Ambulatory physiological assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder: Integrating passive sensing with experience sampling to measure trauma reactivity [Manuscript under review]. University of North Carolina at Greensboro.
go back to reference Wright, A. G., Aslinger, E. N., Bellamy, B., Edershile, E. A., & Woods, W. C. (2019). Daily stress and hassles. In K. L. Harkness & E. P. Hayden (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of stress and mental health (pp. 27–44). Oxford University Press. Wright, A. G., Aslinger, E. N., Bellamy, B., Edershile, E. A., & Woods, W. C. (2019). Daily stress and hassles. In K. L. Harkness & E. P. Hayden (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of stress and mental health (pp. 27–44). Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Yaremych, H. E., Preacher, K. J., & Hedeker, D. (2023). Centering categorical predictors in multilevel models: Best practices and interpretation. Psychological Methods,28(3), 613.PubMedCrossRef Yaremych, H. E., Preacher, K. J., & Hedeker, D. (2023). Centering categorical predictors in multilevel models: Best practices and interpretation. Psychological Methods,28(3), 613.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Uncovering the Temporal Dynamics of Negative Thought in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
Auteurs
Cameron Pugach
Shae Nester
Blair Wisco
Publicatiedatum
02-04-2025
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Cognitive Therapy and Research
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-025-10608-y