Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by pervasive and impairing patterns of attentional dysregulation, hyperactivity, and/or impulsivity, shows substantial overlap with several dimensions of both personality and temperament. All three constructs (ADHD, temperament, and personality) tap aspects of self-regulation, including behavioral and emotional control. Insufficient literature assesses the interplay among these constructs across development. Through the present prospective, longitudinal investigation, we attempt to bridge this gap in knowledge by examining the linkages between childhood ADHD symptoms and adolescent personality dimensions. In so doing, we pay careful attention to methodologic issues that might spuriously inflate such linkages.
ADHD is characterized by two symptom dimensions—inattention (IA) and hyperactivity/ impulsivity (HI)—either (or both) of which may be present in diagnosed ADHD. Inattentive symptoms include behaviors such as making frequent careless mistakes, having difficulty paying attention, getting distracted easily, disorganization, and forgetfulness, whereas hyperactive-impulsive symptoms include frequent fidgeting, climbing or running at inappropriate times, difficulty working or playing quietly, and frequently interrupting (American Psychiatric Association,
2022). Like the categorical ADHD diagnosis, the two symptom dimensions of IA and HI are substantially heritable (Faraone & Larsson,
2019), usually emerge in childhood, and often become evident in the context of schooling.
In contrast, personality traits refer to a person’s habitual patterns of thinking, feeling, and behaving (John,
2021). They are somewhat less heritable
1 than ADHD symptom dimensions and appear to develop out of transactions between youth and their environments from early childhood through adolescence and even early adulthood (Roberts et al.,
2006). Thus, personality traits may be more susceptible to environmental influence (Srivastava et al.,
2003)—and potentially modifiable—than ADHD per se or its symptom dimensions, especially during early and middle adolescence (Soto et al.,
2011).
Here we focus on the personality traits defined by the Big Five taxonomy, which posits five fundamental, bipolar factors: (a) Conscientiousness, encompassing traits such as being responsible, organized, and persistent; (b) Agreeableness, including compassion, respect, and trust; (c) Neuroticism, referring to tendencies toward anxiety, depression, and mood volatility; (d) Openness to Experience, encompassing curiosity and a tendency for aesthetic appreciation; and (e) Extraversion, involving being outgoing and assertive (John et al.,
2008; McCrae & Costa,
1999; Soto & John,
2017). Prior research suggests that three of the Big Five personality domains—specifically Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism—are consistently linked to ADHD and its symptom dimensions. In contrast, examinations of Openness to Experience and Extraversion do not reveal consistent associations, with some variability depending on participant age, sample type (community versus clinical), other participant characteristics, and methodology (Gomez & Corr,
2014).
Although research on personality and developmental psychopathology has often proceeded independently, previous cross-sectional research suggests substantial overlap between measures of personality and ADHD (De Pauw & Mervielde,
2010; Gomez & Corr,
2014). For example, some items on the Big Five Inventory (BFI; John & Srivastava,
1999) assess aspects of task focus and distractibility that are similar to the IA symptoms listed in the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM; American Psychiatric Association,
2022). A core question is whether childhood ADHD symptom dimensions are related to adolescent personality constructs, especially when similar items from measures of either domain are removed and when informants differ across age.
Personality and ADHD: Prior Research
Numerous studies have shown that ADHD—especially IA symptoms—is substantially negatively correlated with the Big Five dimension of Conscientiousness, with correlation sizes estimated to be at least medium-to-large
2 (
r = -0.43 for ADHD,
r = -0.52 for IA, and
r = -0.40 for HI; see meta-analytic review by Gomez & Corr,
2014). In addition, ADHD—especially HI symptoms—is associated with various interpersonal difficulties, including interrupting others, higher levels of peer conflict, and aggression (Gardner & Gerdes,
2015; Hinshaw,
2018). Correspondingly, several studies have shown that ADHD (i.e., HI) negatively correlates with Agreeableness in the Big Five taxonomy, with correlations estimated to range from medium-to-large (
r = -0.31 for ADHD,
r = -0.22 for IA, and
r = -0.30 for HI; Gomez & Corr,
2014).
Moreover, although emotional dysregulation (often manifesting as “emotional impulsivity”) is not part of the DSM diagnosis of ADHD, it is highly linked to the ADHD syndrome (see Barkley,
2015; Faraone et al.,
2019). Regarding the Big Five, this association has been demonstrated in the positive correlations between Neuroticism and ADHD—including its two symptom dimensions, IA and HI, with associations ranging from 0.39 for ADHD, 0.35 for IA, and 0.18 for HI (thus, in the medium to large range; see Gomez & Corr,
2014). Finally, Gomez and Corr’s (
2014) meta-analysis suggests no significant associations between ADHD, IA, or HI and Openness to Experience nor between ADHD, IA, or HI and Extraversion.
The meta-analysis by Gomez and Corr (
2014) spans clinical, community, adult, and child samples, with the strong suggestion that associations between ADHD symptoms and personality traits (i.e., Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism) are present across different samples and age groups. In general, the correlations between ADHD and these three Big Five dimensions tend to be somewhat larger in (a) clinical samples than in community samples and (b) in child and adolescent samples than in adult samples. In their conclusions, Gomez and Corr (
2014) emphasize one important limitation of their meta-analysis: Most of the research available for their review used cross-sectional designs and adult participants. Even though the link between ADHD and personality is an inherently developmental issue, few studies have examined these links longitudinally.
A notable exception is the investigation of Miller et al. (
2008), who examined prospective links between a childhood ADHD diagnosis and self-rated personality in adolescence. Here, personality was measured using the revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R; Costa & McCrae,
1992) and childhood ADHD diagnostic status was assessed via: (1) a screener using teacher-ratings on the IOWA Conners (Loney & Milich,
1982) inattention/overactivity scale, followed by (2) a parent-reported Diagnostic Interview for Children (DISC; Shaffer et al.,
1989) and Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach,
1991) for children who crossed the clinical cutoff on the teacher measure, and (3) clinical classification using DSM-III-R or DSM-IV ADHD criteria. Key findings were that childhood ADHD (categorically defined) was negatively predictive of adolescent Conscientiousness, regardless of the persistence of ADHD into adolescence. On the other hand, childhood ADHD negatively predicted Agreeableness and positively predicted Neuroticism.
Crucially, however, Miller et al. (
2008) examined ADHD solely as a dichotomous variable and did not examine ADHD symptom dimensions separately. Additionally, the entirety of the sample had childhood diagnoses of ADHD Combined Type, limiting generalizability to youth with predominantly inattentive symptoms. Finally, parallel to much research on ADHD and personality, the longitudinal sample in this study consisted primarily of boys (88%), with only 20 girls. Thus, questions remain about the generalizability of such findings to girls with ADHD.
This gap is particularly important because females tend to have higher rates of exclusively inattentive symptoms than boys (Mowlem et al.,
2019), along with lower rates of externalizing comorbidities (Hinshaw et al.,
2022). Note that mean-level sex differences in personality are present as well: Females tend to score higher on Agreeableness, which generally associates with more adaptive development—but also higher on Neuroticism, which could be a risk factor for mental health concerns (McCrae & Costa,
1999). Overall, to better understand ADHD-personality linkages, especially given the longstanding neglect of females in the literature on ADHD, there is a clear need to focus on girls, to deploy longitudinal designs, and to assess not only ADHD diagnostic status but also the two symptom dimensions of IA and HI in the same investigation.
In addition, measurement issues require careful consideration, especially shared method variance, which can artifactually elevate associations between two variables of interest (Campbell & Fiske,
1959). Of foremost importance are (a) using different data sources for the measurement of ADHD and the measurement of personality (i.e., not using self-reports for both) and (b) eliminating any direct item overlap between symptom dimensions and personality scales (e.g., Inattention and Conscientiousness).
Finally, investigators should consider the family backgrounds in which children with or without ADHD are developing, as these contexts have potential implications for personality development. The bulk of personality research in this area has focused on college-student samples, which tend to overrepresent young adults from higher-income families who grew up in what Luthar (
2003) and Korous et al. (
2023) have described as a “culture of affluence”—a context placing pressure on children and adolescents to excel academically. It is important to study ADHD-personality links in samples that are representative of low-income as well as middle- and upper-middle-class families. Through exploratory yet empirically guided examination, we probe whether links between ADHD symptoms in childhood and personality in adolescence might differ in strength as a function of family income—in particular, whether these links are actually weaker in lower-income families (compared to higher-income families, in which pressures for achievement are likely to be quite salient). Specifically, we examine whether children who have childhood ADHD symptoms and grow up in higher-income families feel worse about themselves in adolescence than those from lower-income families.
The Present Research Questions
The current investigation was designed to examine the prospective link between childhood IA and HI (as well as the diagnostic category of ADHD per se) and adolescent personality traits, using different data sources at different time points to minimize shared method variance. Our sample provides a unique opportunity for such examination in females, who are typically understudied in the area of ADHD (Hinshaw et al.,
2022). Based on existing literature, we hypothesize that childhood IA, HI, and categorical ADHD diagnosis will all (a) negatively predict adolescent Conscientiousness and Agreeableness and (b) positively predict adolescent Neuroticism. Given the lack of cross-sectional correlations between ADHD and either Openness to Experience or Extraversion (Gomez & Corr,
2014), we anticipate no significant longitudinal predictions to these two Big Five dimensions.
More specifically, we expect that only IA (and not HI) symptoms will predict adolescent Conscientiousness when ADHD diagnosis is statistically taken into account. Conversely, we also predict that only HI (and not IA) symptoms will predict Agreeableness beyond the effects of ADHD diagnosis. Any prospective effects related to Neuroticism are of real theoretical interest, because emotional instability or dysregulation is not part of the definition and measurement of ADHD, raising the possibility that higher levels of Neuroticism (anxiety, depression, negative self-concept) in adolescence may be a consequence of growing up with ADHD rather than serving as a preexisting disposition in childhood. Finally, regarding family financial standing, we examined whether a “culture of affluence” (Luthar,
2003) may amplify these kinds of effects. In other words, we suspect that linkages between childhood ADHD and negative self-views in adolescence may be stronger for families of higher socioeconomic status. Finally, we assiduously removed any directly overlapping items from our ADHD and personality measures, as failing to do so could spuriously inflate predictive associations.