A dual-component model
Recent theories from diverse fields suggest that mindfulness has a bifactor structure. These theories propose two distinct components that are somewhat similar to those discussed here, which interact synergistically. For example, Kudesia’s (
2019) metacognitive practice formulation focuses on mindfulness training in organizations. It conceptualizes mindfulness by combining
mindful attention, which underscores the initial perceptual stage of the sequential process of mindfulness, with
mindful conceptualization, which centers on the subsequent conceptual phase in this process. Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT; Lindsay & Creswell,
2017) consists of two main principles or tenets. The first tenet asserts that the cultivation of
attention monitoring abilities serves to heighten one's awareness of the current moment, whereas the second tenet asserts that the development of
acceptance skills changes how individuals interact with their present-moment experiences. MAT further posits that mindfulness training effects arise from the synergy between monitoring and acceptance. The Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory (MMT; Garland et al.,
2015) suggests that mindfulness enhances
interoceptive attention and weakens biases associated with the perceptual process, thus enabling individuals to shift their focus from habitual stress evaluations towards a
metacognitive awareness state. According to MMT, this transition leads to an expanded attention to new information, facilitates a reappraisal of one's life circumstances and the savoring of positive experiences, and eventually leads to self-transcendence (Garland & Fredrickson,
2019). Finally, based on traditional Buddhist accounts, Dreyfus (
2011) distinguished between
mindfulness proper, which refers to the fundamental capacity of the mind to remain focused on its object without drifting, and
clear comprehension, which represents the higher-level skill of monitoring one's mental states. According to this perspective, only by combining the ability to retain focus with the ability to employ clear comprehension, it is possible to fully grasp the comprehensive nature of mindfulness.
The components of mindfulness proposed by these models do not perfectly align with each other or with those presented here, but as further elaborated below, notable overlap does exist. Importantly, they all seem to differentiate, albeit in different ways, between attentional or perceptual processes, and more complex cognitive mechanisms that facilitate higher-order and more abstract outcomes. Also, consistent with the current perspective, these models underscore the integration of the two distinct processes or facets of mindfulness and their potential for synergistically impact mental functioning. Finally, they all posit that mindfulness extends far beyond a mere practice of non-evaluative engagement with the present moment, aimed primarily at improving distress management.
However, two key elements differentiate the current formulation of mindfulness from earlier ones: Barsalou's (
2003) distinction between attending to external sensory input and attending to internal mental experiences, and the insights and extensive body of research grounded in Construal Level Theory, which serves as the foundation for the present model.
The salutary effects of mindfulness: A two-dimensional, CLT-based perspective
CLT proposes that low-level and high-level construals serve different cognitive purposes, contingent upon varying levels of psychological distance. In order to represent distal objects, high-level construals have evolved to maintain the essential properties of the reference object. Low-level construals, on the other hand, enable immediate use of objects or current involvement in activities by preserving their details (Trope & Liberman,
2010). Low-level construals reflect the present, while high-level construals transcend it; a concrete mode of thinking highlights differences (e.g., among objects or situations), while an abstract mode of thinking emphasizes higher-level, inclusive categories. We argue that the benefits of mindfulness stem from combining the advantages associated with each of these two seemingly incompatible processes. The present model of mindfulness further suggests that this unique combination enables mitigating the perils associated with each type of cognitive functioning.
Decreased psychological distance from external experiences. Because the state of mindfulness is characterized by decreased psychological distance from external experiences and low construal of them, it allows practitioners to be in touch with the present moment in all its fullness and richness (Shapiro et al.,
2006). This aspect of mindfulness facilitates a focus on the specific and contextualized aspects of situations, enabling people to respond to them more appropriately. Also, as suggested by the MMT (Garland et al.,
2015), when people are attuned to their direct experience, they notice that when their minds wander they lose contact with the present, which distorts and limits their perception (Williams,
2008). Indeed, a wealth of research suggests that overlooking lower-level contextual factors can lead to a variety of biases (Gilbert & Wilson,
2007; Kahneman et al.,
2006). For example, people typically overestimate how they will react to both positive and negative events (Wilson et al.,
2000). Similarly, Nussbaum et al. (
2006) showed that in the context of high psychological distance, confidence in one’s abilities (i.e., a high-level construal) may lead to unfounded performance predictions, which do not take into account circumstantial, lower-level factors (e.g., task difficulty).
Other theoretical perspectives on mindfulness further highlight the beneficial effects of close attention to the present moment. According to MAT, for example, by increasing one's focus on the current experience, training in attention monitoring might facilitate the more frequent and heightened awareness of positive stimuli (Lindsay & Creswell,
2017). Taking a similar perspective, MMT (Garland & Fredrickson,
2019) underscores the significance of savoring, which entails not only focusing on the most prominent sensory aspects of objects or events, but also acknowledging their subtler characteristics and effects. This broadens the variety and depth of feelings and sensations that can be derived from experiences.
Low psychological distance, however, can also render individuals more susceptible to impulsive behavior, habitual responses and cravings triggered by local temptations (Fujita,
2008). We argue that by simultaneously maintaining a high psychological distance from internal mental events, mindfulness practitioners are able to observe external events from a low psychological distance, but without hindering self-control or facilitating maladaptive mental or behavioral responses.
Increased psychological distance from internal experiences. In terms of CLT, taking an active and abstract view of internal mental events (for example, through the use of affect labeling; Lieberman et al.,
2011) facilitates a higher level of psychological construal of these events (Trope & Liberman,
2010). While practicing mindfulness, one endeavors to experience unwelcome thoughts, feelings, or urges that arise rather than avoid them (e.g., by using acceptance strategies; cf. Lindsay & Creswell,
2017), but in a distanced manner – without becoming immersed in them or acting on them. Similarly, adopting an accepting attitude toward positive experiences implies that such experiences are not eagerly sought or clung to when they arise, but rather they are observed and welcomed as they naturally unfold and recede (Lindsay & Creswell,
2017).
In alignment with the present perspective, Garland et al. (
2015) proposed that mindfulness not only fosters close attention to sensory experiences but also facilitates decentering—a greater psychological distance—from one's thoughts and emotions. This process liberates awareness from rigidly structured narratives concerning the self and the world, and promotes the ability to make flexible and adaptive choices in responding to various situations (see also Bernstein et al.,
2015). Thus, by maintaining a high psychological distance from their mental experiences, mindfulness enables practitioners to reduce the emotional and somatic arousal that may be associated with a concrete processing mode (Kross et al.,
2005) and with maintaining a close psychological distance from one’s sensory input (Desrosiers et al.,
2013).
CLT may further elucidate why maintaining greater psychological distance and higher levels of construal regarding one's internal mental experiences can bring about notable benefits. For example, Garland et al. (
2015) proposed that mindfulness facilitates positive reappraisal, which entails expanding the scope of perspective to recognize that even challenging experiences have the potential to serve as catalysts for personal development and growth. CLT posits that abstract representations are simpler and more coherent because they omit or assimilate irrelevant or incidental details, and place the stimuli within a broader context (Trope & Liberman,
2010). Crucially, though, rather than simply being more ambiguous or vague than lower level construals, higher level construals provide
additional information about the stimulus, its meaning, and its interconnections with other stimuli. Accordingly, concrete representations can be abstracted in various ways. For example, "reading a book" may be construed as "acquiring knowledge" or as "having fun". The higher-level construal encompasses aspects that are not readily apparent in the lower-level representation (e.g., the purpose of the action), and places the activity within a larger framework by defining its relationships with other activities and objects (e.g., "attending school" versus "playing outside", respectively). Similarly, an object (e.g., a bicycle) can be abstracted in several ways (e.g., as a means of transportation or as a form of exercise), and therefore be connected to different classes of objects (vehicles or fitness equipment, respectively). Thus, CLT outlines how the abstraction of appraisals, thoughts, feelings, and urges yields further insights into these internal experiences, facilitating their integration into wider and more diverse mental frameworks. For example, negative thoughts and feelings stemming from adverse experiences (e.g., a personal failure) can be seen not just in their immediate negative light, but also as opportunities for future growth and improvement (cf. Garland et al.,
2015).
CLT's proposition that abstraction and higher-level construals enhance our understanding of stimuli and their relationships clarifies broader and more profound favorable outcomes associated with mindfulness. Dreyfus (
2011) suggested that mindfulness “expands the scope of attention so that one becomes aware of the characteristics of experience” (p. 49), thus promoting the development of
clear comprehension, which he describes as “the meta-attentive ability to monitor one’s mental states” (p. 50). In accordance with CLT, which suggests that higher-level construals provide greater insight into stimuli and their relationships, by viewing mental experiences in an abstract and distanced manner—rather than becoming immersed in them—practitioners are able to assess these experiences thoroughly and insightfully. Practicing mindfulness promotes the recognition of habitual patterns (Brown et al.,
2007), distorted cognitions (Baer,
2003) and their antecedents and consequences (Gunaratana,
2010). Ultimately, the cultivation of this meta-cognitive ability fosters the development of a deeper understanding of one's own mind and a clearer comprehension of how it works (Dreyfus,
2011).
Morality and ethical behavior. Garland et al. (
2015) posited that mindfulness may cultivate active involvement in a valued and purpose-driven life by encouraging the development of positive reinterpretations and emotions, thereby engendering profound eudaimonic significance. Values and ethical principles are commonly viewed as abstract cognitive structures that provide meaning and continuity to people despite the changing circumstances surrounding them (Feather,
1995; Rohan,
2000), and as guides for action across situations (Schwartz & Bilsky,
1987). For example, “Thou shalt not kill” is an abstract moral principle believed to be applicable to all contexts (e.g., Graham et al.,
2009; see Alper,
2020). Research indicates that in line with CLT, core values and ethical standards, which are decontextualized and abstract in nature, are more likely to influence people's behavioral choices in the context of high psychological distance and an abstract set of mind (Choi et al.,
2012; Eyal et al.,
2008). Conversely, when psychological distance is low and the mindset is concrete, there tends to be a diminished focus on such abstract priorities, with more attention directed toward immediate, local concerns. However, this shift in focus is not necessarily undesirable.
High psychological distance enhances the effect of high-level internal construals such as ethical beliefs and personal values, and decreases the effect of low-level, contextual factors. Nonetheless, external context can be quite crucial (Bo O’Connor & Fowler,
2023). Taking Alper’s (
2020) example, it is generally wrong to kill, but many will acknowledge that killing in self-defense may not be sinful. People do not necessarily adhere to deontological thinking, i.e., judging solely based on values without considering certain low-level, specific circumstances (Holyoak & Powell,
2016). Depending upon the particular aspects of a particular situation, an action that is generally regarded as a moral transgression may be deemed acceptable. Indeed, there has been considerable evidence that high psychological distance and abstract construals can impact moral judgments, but other studies have shown that local concerns can also play a role (e.g., Gong & Medin,
2012; Žeželj & Jokić,
2014; for a review see Alper,
2020). Thus, CLT suggests that high psychological distance and high-level construals increase awareness of consistency and similarities across different contexts, thereby enabling people to act according to their ethical principles, values and interests, regardless of the nuances of current circumstances. Low psychological distance, on the other hand, elicits concrete, contextual and detail-oriented low-level construals. These low construals are closely attuned to the immediate present, thus allowing people to respond flexibly to the contingencies of the current situation, and in accordance with their local needs and concerns.
The two-dimensional model presented here posits that mindfulness combines both high and low psychological distance from present experiences, thereby allowing practitioners to benefit from both worlds. High-level construal of internal mental experiences, which allows people to transcend habitual tendencies, urges and impulses, facilitates behavior that reflects their core values, ethical principles and moral beliefs. As these abstract cognitive structures become more salient, self-regulated, thoughtful behavior becomes more likely (Graham et al.,
2009). The low psychological distance from external experiences and the outside world enables people to do so in a flexible, non-dogmatic manner, which is responsive to concrete concerns and current circumstances (Alper,
2020).