Skip to main content

Welkom bij Scalda & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Scalda heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen.Je kunt de producten hieronder links aanschaffen en rechts inloggen.

Registreer

Schaf de BSL Academy aan: 

BSL Academy mbo AG

Eenmaal aangeschaft kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met Jan van der Velden.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

08-01-2019 | Original Article

End-state comfort meets pre-crastination

Auteurs: David A. Rosenbaum, Kyle S. Sauerberger

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Research on motor planning has revealed two seemingly contradictory phenomena. One is the end-state comfort effect, the tendency to grasp objects in physically awkward ways for the sake of comfortable or easy-to-control final postures (Rosenbaum et al., Attention and Performance XIII: Motor representation and control, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1990). The other is pre-crastination, the tendency to hasten the completion of tasks even at the expense of extra physical effort (Rosenbaum et al., Psychol Sci 25:1487–1496, 2014). End-state comfort seems to reflect emphasis on final states, whereas pre-crastination seems to reflect emphasis on initial states. How can both effects exist? We sought to resolve this seeming conflict by noting, first, that the effects have been tested in different contexts. End-state comfort has been tested with grasping, whereas pre-crastination has been tested with walking plus grasping. Second, both effects may reflect planning that aids aiming, as already demonstrated for end-state comfort but not yet tested for pre-crastination. We tested the two effects in a single walk-and-grasp task and found that demands on aiming influenced both effects, although precrastination was not fully influenced by changes in the demands of aiming. We conclude that end-state comfort and precrastination are both aiming-related, but that precrastination also reflects a desire to hasten early task completion.
Voetnoten
1
In their review, Rosenbaum et al. (2012) refrained from speaking about the end-state comfort effect per se, except to note, as we do here, that, for better or worse, the term end-state comfort has stuck and continues to be used to refer to second-order grasp planning manifested as physically awkward initial grasps (e.g., Scharoun et al., 2018). We include the term “end-state comfort” in the title of this article to help identify the subject matter of our investigation.
 
2
We have no independent evidence that aiming difficulty was greater for bucket lifting than for bucket lowering, though it was obvious from informal observation that bucket-lift times were much shorter than bucket lowering times. It is also worth noting that Augustyn and Rosenbaum (2006) and Walsh and Rosenbaum (2009) showed that people can choose paths that reduce aiming difficulty in manual aiming tasks, and that Rosenbaum, Brach, and Semenov (2011) and Rosenbaum (2012) showed that people can choose paths that reduce aiming difficulty in combined manual aiming and walking tasks.
 
3
We tested more participants here than is typical of experiments of this general kind. Rosenbaum, Gong, and Potts (2014) tested 27 participants in each of their experiments, for example. The reason we tested more participants is that all of the present participants also took personality inventories which we planned to use to relate the propensity for pre-crastination to individual differences. Large sample sizes are needed to properly power personality studies (Fraley & Vazire, 2014). The original plan for Experiment 1 was to obtain personality and performance data from 100 participants, but scheduling difficulties prevented us from meeting this goal. As a result, we could only test n = 87 participants. Our target value of n = 100 was met in Experiment 2, however. The results of the personality inventories regarding the performance data will be reported in another article which will also include other experiments (Sauerberger, Funder, & Rosenbaum, 2018).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Archibald, S. J., Mateer, C. A., & Kerns, K. A. (2001). Utilization behavior: Clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 117–130.CrossRefPubMed Archibald, S. J., Mateer, C. A., & Kerns, K. A. (2001). Utilization behavior: Clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 117–130.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Augustyn, J. S., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2006). Metacognitive control of action: Preparation for aiming reflects knowledge of Fitts’ Law. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 911–916.CrossRef Augustyn, J. S., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2006). Metacognitive control of action: Preparation for aiming reflects knowledge of Fitts’ Law. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 911–916.CrossRef
go back to reference Cowie, D., Smith, L., & Braddick, O. (2010). The development of locomotor planning for end-state comfort. Perception, 39, 661–670.CrossRefPubMed Cowie, D., Smith, L., & Braddick, O. (2010). The development of locomotor planning for end-state comfort. Perception, 39, 661–670.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Prospective memory: Multiple retrieval processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 286–290.CrossRef Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Prospective memory: Multiple retrieval processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 286–290.CrossRef
go back to reference Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381.CrossRefPubMed Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fournier, L. R., Coder, E., Kogan, C., Raghunath, N., Taddese, E., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2018a). Which task will we choose first? Precrastination and cognitive load in task ordering. Attention, Perception & Performance. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-1633-5 (Published on-line in advance of paper publication).CrossRef Fournier, L. R., Coder, E., Kogan, C., Raghunath, N., Taddese, E., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2018a). Which task will we choose first? Precrastination and cognitive load in task ordering. Attention, Perception & Performance. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3758/​s13414-018-1633-5 (Published on-line in advance of paper publication).CrossRef
go back to reference Fraley, R. C., & Vazire, S. (2014). The N-pact factor: Evaluating the quality of empirical journals with respect to sample size and statistical power. PLoS ONE. 9(10). Fraley, R. C., & Vazire, S. (2014). The N-pact factor: Evaluating the quality of empirical journals with respect to sample size and statistical power. PLoS ONE. 9(10).
go back to reference Herbort, O., Mathew, H., & Kunde, W. (2017). Habit outweighs planning in grasp selection for object manipulation. Cognitive Psychology, 92, 127–140.CrossRefPubMed Herbort, O., Mathew, H., & Kunde, W. (2017). Habit outweighs planning in grasp selection for object manipulation. Cognitive Psychology, 92, 127–140.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hughes, C. M., Seegelke, C., & Schack, T. (2012). The influence of initial and final precision on motor planning: individual differences in end-state comfort during unimanual grasping and placing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 44, 195–201.CrossRefPubMed Hughes, C. M., Seegelke, C., & Schack, T. (2012). The influence of initial and final precision on motor planning: individual differences in end-state comfort during unimanual grasping and placing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 44, 195–201.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.CrossRef
go back to reference Luce, R. D. (1959). Individual Choice Behavior: A Theoretical Analysis. New York: Wiley. Luce, R. D. (1959). Individual Choice Behavior: A Theoretical Analysis. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.CrossRefPubMed Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Potts, C. A., Brown, A. A., Solnik, S., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2017). A method for measuring manual positioning control. Acta Psychologica, 180, 117–121.CrossRefPubMed Potts, C. A., Brown, A. A., Solnik, S., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2017). A method for measuring manual positioning control. Acta Psychologica, 180, 117–121.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rayburn-Reeves, R. M., Molet, M., & Zentall, T. R. (2011). Simultaneous discrimination reversal learning in pigeons and humans: Anticipatory and perseverative errors. Learning & Behavior, 39, 125–137.CrossRef Rayburn-Reeves, R. M., Molet, M., & Zentall, T. R. (2011). Simultaneous discrimination reversal learning in pigeons and humans: Anticipatory and perseverative errors. Learning & Behavior, 39, 125–137.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A. (2012). The tiger on your tail: Choosing between temporally extended behaviors. Psychological Science, 23, 855–860.CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum, D. A. (2012). The tiger on your tail: Choosing between temporally extended behaviors. Psychological Science, 23, 855–860.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A. (2017). Knowing Hands—The Cognitive Psychology of Manual Control. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Rosenbaum, D. A. (2017). Knowing Hands—The Cognitive Psychology of Manual Control. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Brach, M., & Semenov, A. (2011). Behavioral ecology meets motor behavior: Choosing between walking and reaching paths. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43, 131–136.CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Brach, M., & Semenov, A. (2011). Behavioral ecology meets motor behavior: Choosing between walking and reaching paths. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43, 131–136.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Chapman, K. M., Weigelt, M., Weiss, D. J., & van der Wel, R. (2012). Cognition, action, and object manipulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 924–946.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rosenbaum, D. A., Chapman, K. M., Weigelt, M., Weiss, D. J., & van der Wel, R. (2012). Cognition, action, and object manipulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 924–946.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Gong, L., & Potts, C. A. (2014). Pre-crastination: Hastening subgoal completion at the expense of extra physical effort. Psychological Science, 25, 1487–1496.CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Gong, L., & Potts, C. A. (2014). Pre-crastination: Hastening subgoal completion at the expense of extra physical effort. Psychological Science, 25, 1487–1496.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Halloran, E., & Cohen, R. G. (2006). Grasping movement plans. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 918–922.CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Halloran, E., & Cohen, R. G. (2006). Grasping movement plans. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 918–922.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J., & Jorgensen, M. (1990). Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance XIII: Motor representation and control (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J., & Jorgensen, M. (1990). Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance XIII: Motor representation and control (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., van Heugten, C., & Caldwell, G. C. (1996). From cognition to biomechanics and back: The end-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. Acta Psychologica, 94, 59–85.CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., van Heugten, C., & Caldwell, G. C. (1996). From cognition to biomechanics and back: The end-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. Acta Psychologica, 94, 59–85.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Vaughan, J., Jorgensen, M. J., Barnes, H. J., & Stewart, E. (1993). Plans for object manipulation. In D. E. Meyer & S. Kornblum (Eds.), Attention and performance XIV—A silver jubilee: Synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence and cognitive neuroscience (pp. 803–820). Cambridge: MIT Press, Bradford Books. Rosenbaum, D. A., Vaughan, J., Jorgensen, M. J., Barnes, H. J., & Stewart, E. (1993). Plans for object manipulation. In D. E. Meyer & S. Kornblum (Eds.), Attention and performance XIV—A silver jubilee: Synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence and cognitive neuroscience (pp. 803–820). Cambridge: MIT Press, Bradford Books.
go back to reference Sauerberger, K. S., Funder, D. C., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2018). Pre-crastination as an individual difference (in preparation) Sauerberger, K. S., Funder, D. C., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2018). Pre-crastination as an individual difference (in preparation)
go back to reference Scharoun, S. M., Gonzalez, D. A., Roy, E. A., & Bryden, P. J. (2018). End-state comfort across the lifespan: A cross-sectional investigation of how movement context influences motor planning in an overturned glass task. Motor Control, 22, 211–230.CrossRefPubMed Scharoun, S. M., Gonzalez, D. A., Roy, E. A., & Bryden, P. J. (2018). End-state comfort across the lifespan: A cross-sectional investigation of how movement context influences motor planning in an overturned glass task. Motor Control, 22, 211–230.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Short, M. W., & Cauraugh, J. (1999). Precision hypothesis and the end-state comfort effect. Acta Psychologica, 100, 243–252.CrossRefPubMed Short, M. W., & Cauraugh, J. (1999). Precision hypothesis and the end-state comfort effect. Acta Psychologica, 100, 243–252.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Siegel, S., & Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences (Second Ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Siegel, S., & Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences (Second Ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
go back to reference Solnik, S., Pazin, N., Coelho, C. J., Rosenbaum, D. A., Scholz, J. P., Zatsiorksy, V. M., & Latash, M. L. (2013). End-state comfort and joint configuration variance during reaching. Experimental Brain Research, 225, 431–442.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Solnik, S., Pazin, N., Coelho, C. J., Rosenbaum, D. A., Scholz, J. P., Zatsiorksy, V. M., & Latash, M. L. (2013). End-state comfort and joint configuration variance during reaching. Experimental Brain Research, 225, 431–442.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Walsh, M. M., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2009). Deciding how to act is not achieved by watching mental movies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1481–1489.PubMed Walsh, M. M., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2009). Deciding how to act is not achieved by watching mental movies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1481–1489.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
End-state comfort meets pre-crastination
Auteurs
David A. Rosenbaum
Kyle S. Sauerberger
Publicatiedatum
08-01-2019
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-01142-6