Main Findings
In this retrospective population-based cohort study, we examined the association between threatened miscarriage and ASD and ADHD by age 14 years. We have yielded two principal findings. First, threatened miscarriage was associated with a 55% increase in the likelihood of ASD. Second, threatened miscarriage was associated with a 51% increase in the likelihood of ADHD. These associations remained significant after controlling for several potential confounders including maternal age, maternal education, household income, maternal smoking status, maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy, pre-pregnancy body mass index, depression or mental illness during pregnancy, infant sex, and parity.
In agreement with this data, other published studies (Glasson et al.,
2004; Kolevzon et al.,
2007; Autism Spectrum Disorder,
2018) support these findings, reporting that haemorrhage without cervix dilation within the first 20 weeks of gestation is a commonly reported symptom among ASD/ADHD case mothers.
Furthermore, congruent with other research, our findings observed a significant association between threatened miscarriage and ASD and ADHD among male infants. (
Centers for Disease Control Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring (ADDM) Network, 2018; Carlsson et al.,
2021) While the association between threatened miscarriage and ASD among females was not statistically significant for ASD, and reduced odds were demonstrated for ADHD, some studies theorise that NDDs in females might be underdiagnosed, potentially biasing the results towards the null. (Carlsson et al.,
2021). Furthermore, we found some evidence exposure to threatened miscarriage and being born SGA was associated with an increased likelihood of an ASD diagnosis. Both vaginal bleeding and SGA are associated with placental dysfunction. (Saraswat et al.,
2010; Miller et al.,
2016; Maher et al.,
2019) Placental dysfunction impacts the adequate supply of nutrients and oxygen provided to the baby and the removal of harmful waste from the baby. As the central organ responsible for the regulation of these maternal and foetal interactions, a compromised placenta fails to properly support the developing foetus and is therefore hypothesised to be a cause of adverse neurodevelopmental consequences for a developing brain. (Miller et al.,
2016).
Strengths and Limitations
The current study has several strengths. First, we used data from a large, nationally representative cohort of children born in the UK between 2000 and 2002. Second, this cohort contains access to a wide variety of information including economic, sociodemographic, and perinatal variables which have been inconsistently accounted for in previous studies. (Saraswat et al.,
2010; Wang et al.,
2017). This allowed for the adjustment of many potential confounders such as maternal age, maternal education, household income, maternal smoking status, maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy, pre-pregnancy body mass index, depression or mental illness during pregnancy, infant sex, and parity. Further, the data allowed us to conduct sensitivity analyses for variables that could be considered as mediators or potential confounders, by stratifying for those with and without hypertensive disorders of pregnancy (HDP), those with and without being born small for gestational age (SGA), and born preterm, we also stratified the results among those ever diagnosed with maternal depression or anxiety.
The study also has some limitations that should be noted. First, data on potential confounders were self-reported at nine months postpartum and therefore subject to recall bias. However, the agreement between self-reported lifestyle factors such as smoking pre-pregnancy and during pregnancy with antenatal records has been shown to be good very good 4–9 years post-delivery.(Rice et al.,
2007; Skulstad et al.,
2017). Additionally, as data on potential confounders were collected 9 months post-delivery in the current study, it is likely to be more accurate than data collected 4–9 years after pregnancy. Second, threatened miscarriage was also subject to recall bias. However, when measuring our exposure, mothers were asked if their illness or problem during pregnancy required medical attention and treatment. The requirement for medical attention/treatment may improve recall as the event is likely more salient to the individual. Furthermore, the rate of exposure observed in the current study (6.0%) is somewhat comparable to that reported in a Danish registry-based cohort which used ICD-coding to classify threatened miscarriage (Dudukina et al.,
2021). Nevertheless, the rate of exposure observed in the current study (6.0%) may be underestimated compared to previously reported estimates of threatened miscarriage of up to 20% (Sharma et al.,
2020). However, any misclassification of the exposure is likely to be non-differential and therefore potentially biasing results towards the null.
Third, reporting of ASD and ADHD within this cohort is also based solely on parental recall which could potentially introduce misclassification of our outcomes. (Kolevzon et al.,
2007). However, it is not uncommon to see parent-reported neurodevelopmental disorders being used in previously published research. (Rice et al.,
2007; Blumberg et al.,
2013; Russell et al.,
2014; Curran et al.,
2016; Skulstad et al.,
2017) Furthermore, as parents were unaware of the current study hypothesis at the time of data collection, any misclassification would likely be non-differential.
Fourth, the case assessment diagnosis for both neurodevelopmental disorders in this study did not include confirmation from the gold standard measurement tools such as the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) or the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). However, the prevalence of ASD within this cohort (3.7%) is comparable to that reported in English primary care data for 2020–2021 (O’Nions et al.,
2023) and is somewhat comparable to recent data figures published by the Centers for Disease Control Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring (ADDM) Network,
2020. Likewise, the prevalence of parent-reported ADHD within this cohort (3.2%) is similar to UK-rates reported elsewhere (National Health Service,
2018), however it is lower compared to estimates reported in the United States. (Russell et al.,
2014; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD),
2019). This discrepancy could be due to differing diagnostic criteria and cultural practices when considering the label of ADHD or, albeit less likely, due to truly lower levels of ADHD symptoms in the UK. (Russell et al.,
2014). Fifth, there is a lack of data on family history of ASD and ADHD diagnosis, and genetic sequencing within the participants of the cohort is not available, therefore, these variables could not be controlled for as confounding factors within the analyses. It has been established from previous studies and in clinical settings, that ASD and ADHD are high heritability disorders owing to inherited genetic influences, such that, ASD is estimated at 80% and ADHD at 74%, respectively. (Bai et al.,
2019; Faraone & Larsson,
2019). Therefore, unmeasured confounding, including confounding due to shared genetics, cannot be ruled out. However, the relatively high E-values suggest the observed associations may be less likely to be explained away by unmeasured confounding factors. Finally, loss to follow-up may have introduced selection bias in the current study. For example, our sample size included 18,294 singleton mother-child pairs at wave 1. This was reduced to 15,590 in wave 2, 15,246 in wave 3, 13,857 in wave 4, 13, 287 in wave 5, and 11,726 in wave 6. As participation in follow-up research can be influenced by several factors, and children with behavioural disorders are more likely to be lost to follow-up, this may have biased our results towards the null (Wolke et al.,
2009).