Psychological theories of vulnerabilities for depressive symptoms and disorders have proliferated for over 40 years. The list of potentially important vulnerabilities now includes personality traits such as neuroticism, personality-cognitive styles such as sociotropy and autonomy, and cognitive styles such as dysfunctional attitudes and negative inferential style. Significant concurrent relationships have been found between depressive symptoms and neuroticism (e.g., Klein et al.
2009), dysfunctional attitudes (reviewed in Clark and Beck
1999), negative inferential style (e.g., Haeffel et al.
2008), sociotropy (e.g., Robins et al.
1994), and autonomy (e.g., Zuroff
1994). Additional research has supported the validity of these constructs in prospective prediction of depressive symptoms and disorders (Alloy et al.
2006; Clark and Beck
1999; Clark et al.
1994; Klein et al.
2009).
The current study focuses on three important issues in this area of research that have received relatively little attention. First, although these theories of cognitive diatheses and personality-cognitive styles were originally hypothesized as vulnerabilities to depressive symptoms and disorders, little research has examined whether these are also diatheses for closely related (and highly comorbid) anxiety symptoms and disorders (e.g., Clark et al.
1994; Mineka et al.
1998). Second, relatively few investigations have examined these specific diatheses collectively to determine which are uniquely associated with depressive (or anxiety) symptoms and disorders. Finally, it is also not known whether these putatively more specific vulnerabilities for depression make unique contributions to symptoms above and beyond neuroticism (N), which has already been well established as a predictor of anxiety and depression (e.g., Clark et al.
1994; Klein et al.
2009; Watson et al.
2005). Thus, the purpose of the present study was to examine these three issues in a cross-sectional sample of high school juniors participating in the Youth Emotion Project—an ongoing longitudinal study of common and specific risk factors for mood and anxiety symptoms and disorders.
Cognitive Vulnerability Models for Depression and Anxiety
Research on cognitive theories of depression has focused primarily on dysfunctional attitudes and negative inferential style. Dysfunctional attitudes were first described by Beck (e.g.,
1967) whose theory posits that maladaptive or dysfunctional attitudes, beliefs, and assumptions about the self, world, and future predispose individuals to experience depression, particularly when these beliefs are activated by negative life events (e.g., Clark and Beck
1999). Abramson, Alloy, and colleagues have investigated a somewhat different cognitive diathesis—negative inferential style (e.g., Alloy et al.
2006). Negative inferential style refers to the tendency to interpret negative life events as leading to negative consequences, signifying fundamental flaws about the self, and having stable and global causes. Dysfunctional attitudes and inferential style are moderately correlated, with studies in unselected college samples reporting correlations between .4 and .5 (e.g., Hankin et al.
2005).
Although both of these cognitive styles were originally proposed as vulnerabilities for depression, researchers have questioned whether these styles might not also apply to anxiety, or to the overlap between anxiety and depression (e.g., Alloy et al.
1990; Mineka et al.
1995,
1998). First, substantial overlap has been documented between anxiety and depression at both the symptom and diagnostic levels (e.g., Clark and Watson
1991). Second, the proponents of these cognitive vulnerability theories have themselves cited the possibility of such relationships. For example, Haeffel et al. (
2008) recently reviewed the literature on the Cognitive Style Questionnaire (CSQ) (e.g., Alloy et al.
2000) and cited a need for further investigation of its specificity to depression. Moreover, Clark and Beck (
1999) noted that the scale used to measure their construct (DAS) is called the “Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale and not the Depressotypic or Depressogenic Attitudes Scale” (p. 137).
Several studies have reported significant relationships between anxiety symptoms and these putatively depressogenic vulnerabilities, although most studies have focused on only a few types of anxiety symptoms. For example, in a student sample, Hankin et al. (
2004) reported moderate concurrent associations between both the DAS and the CSQ with general anxiety and anxious arousal symptoms assessed by the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (Watson et al.
1995a). Hankin (
2008a) also found moderate correlations between inferential style and anxious arousal symptoms concurrently and longitudinally in a sample of sixth to tenth graders. Finally, Oliver et al. (
2007) reported small to moderate associations between both the DAS and the CSQ with anxiety symptoms on the Beck Anxiety Inventory (Beck et al.
1988). The present study extends these associations to a wider range of anxiety symptomatology.
Personality-Cognitive Styles and Depression and Anxiety
Sociotropy and autonomy are two personality-cognitive style constructs that have been hypothesized to confer vulnerability to depression (e.g., Clark and Beck
1999). Individuals scoring highly on sociotropy are described as basing their self-worth on the closeness and security of social relations with others, whereas those scoring highly on autonomy are described as basing their self-worth on achievement, independence, and control. These two personality-cognitive styles are partially independent, with the two constructs correlating only .20 in two college student samples (Robins et al.
1994).
Although several studies have reported significant main effects of sociotropy and autonomy on concurrent self-reported depression (e.g., Frewen and Dozois
2006; Robins et al.
1994), there has been only limited investigation into whether they are also predictors of anxiety symptoms. For example, Alford and Gerrity (
1995) reported significant concurrent associations between sociotropy (but not autonomy) and symptoms of both depression and anxiety. However, Fresco et al. (
2001) found concurrent associations between sociotropy and anxiety (but not depressive) symptoms, whereas autonomy related only to depressive symptoms. Clearly more research is needed on this topic before drawing any firm conclusions as the empirical work to date is quite limited in depth and scope.
Neuroticism
The present study sought to compare the incremental validity of these putatively more specific diatheses to the broader personality trait of N which has been defined as a relatively stable trait tapping one’s sensitivity to negative stimuli and the tendency to experience negative mood states (e.g., Clark et al.
1994; Eysenck and Eysenck
1975). In a widely cited early review, Clark et al. (
1994) concluded that high N confers increased risk for both anxiety and depression. Over the past 15 years, a significant body of additional research has further documented these associations (see Klein et al.
2009; Watson et al.
2005, for recent reviews).
Incremental Validity of Predictors
As noted, one objective of the present study was to examine the degree to which cognitive style and personality-cognitive vulnerabilities make unique contributions to symptoms of depression and/or anxiety. The present investigation also assessed the incremental validity of these other vulnerabilities to associations with depressive and anxiety symptoms beyond the variance accounted for by N and, conversely, the incremental validity of N beyond these other vulnerabilities (see Paunonen
1998). A number of studies have examined unique and overlapping components of these vulnerabilities (e.g., Dunkley et al.
1997; Hankin et al.
2004,
2005; Mongrain and Blackburn
2005; Oliver et al.
2007). For example, using a longitudinal design, Hankin et al. (
2004, Study 2) found that when the CSQ and the DAS were examined simultaneously in the prediction of depressive symptoms and depressive disorder, neither main effects, nor diathesis-stress interactions (entered on a subsequent step), were uniquely significant for either cognitive style. Thus, they concluded that associations with depression are found in the overlapping components of those cognitive vulnerabilities. Dunkley et al. (
1997) examined the incremental validity of sociotropy, autonomy, and N in associations with depressive symptoms using a cross-sectional design. They reported that when N was entered into equations predicting depressive symptoms, solitude (a facet of autonomy) in men, and sociotropy in women, were no longer significant predictors of symptoms. They concluded that N might account for much of the association between sociotropy, autonomy, and general distress (see also Zuroff et al.
2004a).
To our knowledge, only two other studies have examined all five of these vulnerabilities simultaneously. Mongrain and Blackburn (
2005) examined them in a sample of 97 graduate students who each had a prior history of depressive disorder. They reported that both attributional style and dysfunctional attitudes were uniquely associated with the number of prior depressive episodes above and beyond neuroticism, sociotropy, and autonomy; none of the latter was uniquely significant. Analyses predicting recurrence of depression over the following 16 months demonstrated unique contributions from attributional style and autonomy. No significant relationships were found between cognitive styles and current or past anxiety diagnoses. Our study differs in several important ways from the Mongrain and Blackburn study. First, whereas our study investigates a large community-based adolescent sample, Mongrain and Blackburn had a highly selected sample of graduate students with a history of mood disorder, raising serious questions about the generalizability of their results. Second, our analyses examine five different types of anxiety symptoms separately versus combining them into a single dimension because the strength of associations between these vulnerabilities and anxiety may well differ among types of anxiety (see Mineka et al.
1995). Finally, our study (
n > 500) has substantially greater power to detect small unique effects than did Mongrain and Blackburn (
n = 97).
A second investigation was also conducted on the YEP data set (Zinbarg et al.
2010) with objectives related to those of the present investigation. Using logistic regression, Zinbarg et al. examined the incremental validity of cognitive and personality-cognitive vulnerabilities and N in their relationships with past diagnoses of anxiety and depressive disorders in the same sample of adolescents. Using current depression and/or anxiety symptoms as covariates, Zinbarg et al. (
2010) reported that N was uniquely associated with past mood disorders (although only for men) and past anxiety disorders, while no other vulnerability made a unique contribution.
In contrast to the study by Zinbarg et al. (
2010) which examined only past diagnoses, the present study focuses on current symptoms. This focus on current symptoms of anxiety and depression is important, particularly if the results differ from those found with diagnoses. Such symptom level analyses are consistent with a large number of studies examining cognitive and personality-cognitive vulnerabilities using dimensional models of psychopathology. Further, the present study evaluated specific symptom clusters for different types of mood and anxiety symptoms whereas Zinbarg et al. collapsed outcomes into depressive and/or anxiety disorders because of limited sample sizes for some individual disorders. Moreover, both clinical and sub-clinical levels of depressive symptoms are associated with psychosocial dysfunction (e.g., Lewinsohn et al.
2000) and sub-clinical depressive and anxiety symptoms have been associated with later risk for mood and anxiety disorders in numerous studies (e.g., Fergusson et al.
2005; Murphy et al.
1989; Pine et al.
1999). Thus, even if the relationships between certain putative vulnerabilities and past diagnoses were not significant, a significant association between these same vulnerabilities and current symptoms could still indicate risk for later disorder.
Objectives
The present study had three objectives. First, we examined whether cognitive and personality-cognitive styles theorized to be related to depression were also significantly related to symptoms of different types of anxiety. Two patterns of results could be hypothesized based on past research and theory. A specificity hypothesis would suggest that whereas all five vulnerabilities should be significantly associated with depression, only N should be significantly related to anxiety symptoms. Alternatively, a nonspecificity hypothesis suggests that vulnerabilities for depression may be vulnerabilities for at least some types of anxiety as well.
The second and third objectives examined the incremental validity of the cognitive style, personality-cognitive style, and neuroticism vulnerabilities for each of eight outcomes: two depression scales, a depression symptom composite, and five scales measuring different types of anxiety symptoms. These comparisons initially examined just the four specific vulnerabilities and subsequently included the broader neuroticism vulnerability. If individual vulnerabilities were significant unique predictors of symptom outcomes, it would indicate that meaningful variance was accounted for by the non-overlapping components of each diathesis. Alternatively, if the group of predictors as a whole accounted for significant variance in depressive or anxiety symptoms, but the individual predictors did not, such findings would support the hypothesis that the overlapping aspects of the vulnerabilities were the key to predicting symptoms. Thus, our second and third objectives focused on the degree to which overlapping versus unique aspects of these vulnerabilities were associated with current mood and anxiety symptoms.