Skip to main content

Welkom bij Scalda & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Scalda heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen.Je kunt de producten hieronder links aanschaffen en rechts inloggen.

Registreer

Schaf de BSL Academy aan: 

BSL Academy mbo AG

Eenmaal aangeschaft kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Heb je een vraag, neem dan contact op met Jan van der Velden.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top
Gepubliceerd in:

Open Access 15-02-2025 | ORIGINAL PAPER

TMS-EEG Shows Mindfulness Meditation Is Associated With a Different Excitation/Inhibition Balance in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex

Auteurs: Gregory Humble, Harry Geddes, Oliver Baell, Jake Elijah Payne, Aron T. Hill, Sung Wook Chung, Melanie Emonson, Melissa Osborn, Bridget Caldwell, Paul B. Fitzgerald, Robin Cash, Neil W. Bailey

Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness | Uitgave 2/2025

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Objectives

Mindfulness meditation is associated with functional brain changes in regions subserving higher order cognitive processes such as attention. However, no research to date has probed these areas in meditators using combined transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and electroencephalography (EEG). This study aimed to investigate whether cortical reactivity to TMS differs in a community sample of experienced mindfulness meditators when compared to matched controls.

Method

TMS was applied to the left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (DLPFC) of 19 controls and 15 meditators while brain responses were measured using EEG. TMS-evoked potentials (P60 and N100) were compared between the groups using repeated measures ANOVAs and Mann–Whitney U tests where appropriate, and exploratory analyses using the whole EEG scalp field were performed to test whether TMS-evoked global neural response strength or the distribution of neural activity differed between groups.

Results

Meditators were found to have statistically larger P60/N100 ratios in response to both left and right hemisphere DLPFC stimulation compared to controls (both left and right pFDR < 0.01, BF10 > 39). No differences were observed in P60 or N100 amplitudes when examined independently. We also found preliminary evidence for differences in the spatial distribution of neural activity 269–332 ms post stimulation.

Conclusions

These differences in the distribution of neural activity around 300 ms after stimulation suggest that meditators may have differences in connectivity between cortical and subcortical brain regions, potentially reflecting greater inhibitory activity in frontal regions. This research contributes to our current understanding of the neurophysiology of mindfulness and highlights opportunities for further exploration into the mechanisms underpinning the benefits of mindfulness meditation.

Preregistration

This study is not preregistered.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12671-025-02519-6.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Mindfulness meditation is a form of mental training that has been found to improve several aspects of general wellbeing (Gu et al., 2015; Spijkerman et al., 2016), including mental health outcomes (Braun et al., 2019; Godfrin & van Heeringen, 2010). Previous research indicated that mindfulness meditation is associated with changes in brain structure and function (Fox et al., 2014; Young et al., 2018). These changes include reductions in the age-related decline of grey matter (Pagnoni & Cekic, 2007) as well as improvement in the performance of several cognitive processes (Chiesa & Malinowski, 2011; Tang et al., 2015). In particular, processes related to attention and executive functions have been suggested to be improved by mindfulness meditation (Bailey et al., 2020, 2023b; Wang et al., 2020).
However, while there is a substantial body of evidence indicating that neuropsychological processes are altered by mindfulness meditation, research has yet to fully characterise the underlying neural mechanisms. Theoretical perspectives suggest that mindfulness-based Interventions (MBIs) are more likely to yield positive outcomes post-intervention when they are designed to target the underlying intervention mechanisms, rather than targeting focusing solely on outcome measures (Britton, 2019; Britton et al., 2018). As such, gaining a deeper understanding of the neurophysiological mechanisms that lead to brain changes through the practice of meditation might enable evidence-based modifications to MBIs. These modifications could then directly target specific brain regions, networks and neurophysiological processes implicated in mindfulness practice.
The present study sought to contribute towards a deeper understanding of the structural and functional neural correlates of mindfulness practice. To achieve this, the study combined transcranial magnetic stimulation-electroencephalography (TMS-EEG) to probe neural activity directly. Neuroimaging studies have identified key brain regions associated with enhanced self-regulation in mindfulness meditation. Synthesising this research, one model described by Ganesan et al. (2022) suggests that during focused attention mindfulness meditation, regions responsible for conscious awareness of target interoceptive sensations activate and compete with regions tied to internal thought processes, such as mind wandering. Consequently, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and other executive control regions activate to provide top-down regulation, suppressing the processes that produce mind wandering.
The interactions between different brain regions are crucial for brain function, but another fundamental aspect of neurophysiology is the relative balance between cortical excitatory and inhibitory inputs to cortical pyramidal cells (Zhou & Yu, 2018). At pyramidal cells, neurotransmitters and receptors work together within localised dendritic networks to facilitate the changes that lead to structural and functional modifications (Gulyaeva, 2017). Moreover, the balance of excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission at these neurons plays a key role in creating and maintaining stable oscillatory activity. This activity is not just essential for neuronal signalling, but also underpins higher-order cognitive functions and the brain’s neuroplastic responses to sensory stimuli (Meunier et al., 2017). Therefore, the observed structural and functional changes in meditators might stem from heightened neuroplasticity arising from shifts in the balance of excitatory and inhibitory inputs to cortical circuits.
Potential improvements in mental health due to mindfulness meditation might be linked to differences in excitatory/inhibitory (E/I) ratios within prefrontal cortical regions. However, the exact relationship between cortical E/I ratios and mindfulness meditation remains to be explored. Single-pulse TMS-EEG offers a unique window into this neural activity. Single-pulse TMS-EEG involves using TMS to deliver a targeted magnetic pulse to specific cortical regions of the brain via the scalp and measuring the brain’s response using EEG. This TMS pulse induces transient electrical current in the underlying cortex, leading to the generation of both excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic potentials in stimulated neurons, known as EPSPs and IPSPs respectively, which can be measured using scalp EEG electrodes (for a review, see Farzan et al., 2016). By recording EEG concurrently following the induction of the transient electrical current via TMS, we can observe highly temporally precise information on the cortical response to TMS stimulation. Such data can shed light on the balance and nature of inhibitory and excitatory cortical responses.
When EEG activity is measured after stimulating the DLPFC, reproducible TMS-evoked potentials (TEPs) can be measured. TEPs manifest as complex waveforms with distinct positive peaks and negative troughs at specific post-stimulation latencies. The P60 and N100 are two especially reproducible TEP components that are thought to be associated with glutamatergic excitatory neurotransmission and GABABergic-mediated inhibitory neurotransmission respectively (Cash et al., 2017; Noda et al., 2017; Rogasch & Fitzgerald, 2013; Rogasch et al., 2015). While evidence suggests that glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmissions are not the only factors affecting variations in the P60 and N100, a parsimonious interpretation posits that these components are influenced by both inhibitory and excitatory cortical neural activity, and that TEPs generally reflect cortical reactivity. Given the underlying neural basis for these components, several studies have used the P60 to N100 ratio as a marker of cortical E/I balance (Noda et al., 2017; Voineskos et al., 2019). However, existing research has primarily employed TMS-EEG to assess neural activity in meditators by providing stimulation at the primary motor cortex (M1) site. This research has indicated enhanced GABABergic inhibitory neurotransmission in experienced meditators (Guglietti et al., 2013). The stimulation in these investigations was applied immediately after a 60-min meditation session. As such, the observed effects may be state-dependent rather than trait-based, and therefore may not be informative about the long-term trait effects of meditation on the brain (Tang et al., 2016). Further, the inhibitory transmission changes at M1 in meditators may reflect differences in somatosensory processes, such as changes in self-related interoceptive sensory processing (Tang, 2017), rather than attention- and executive function–related brain regions which are more likely probed by the DLPFC stimulation in the current study.
Considering the provided background, the study’s primary aim was to ascertain if N100 amplitudes differ between experienced mindfulness meditators and non-meditators. A secondary objective was to examine potential differences in the P60 between these two groups. The third aim was to determine if there were noticeable differences in the N100 to P60 ratio when comparing mindfulness meditators with non-meditators. We hypothesised that the N100 TEP component at the DLPFC would show increased amplitudes in experienced mindfulness meditators compared to matched controls reflecting increased inhibitory activity that might underpin the attention enhancements suggested to be associated with the practice of meditation. We also hypothesised that this would coincide with a compensatory increase of the P60 amplitude. While we did not expect that meditators would show a lateralisation effect, we stimulated both the left and right DLPFC to enable us to assess whether any effects were consistent across the different hemispheres of the brain. Furthermore, while we anticipated that meditators might show different E/I balances to non-meditators, we were not aware of research suggesting a specific direction for the potential difference. As such, we had a non-directional hypothesis that the E/I balance (operationally defined as the P60/N100 ratio) would differ in experienced mindfulness meditators when compared to matched controls. In addition to these targeted hypotheses, we conducted exploratory analyses on the EEG data to discern if the global neural response strength or the distribution of active neural sources varied between the two groups.

Method

Participants

Experienced mindfulness meditators and healthy controls between the ages of 18 and 65 were recruited to take part in the study. All participants gave their informed consent prior to participating in any study activities. Participants were recruited through community advertisements, posters and flyers. Advertisements were posted on social media platforms and flyers were distributed at meditation centres in Victoria, Australia, and within the broader community. Participants responded to the advertisements via phone or email and were screened for inclusion/exclusion criteria. Respondents were excluded from the study if they (1) had received a clinical diagnosis of a mental health disorder; (2) had a neurological condition or psychiatric illness (either previously or currently occurring); (3) were currently taking psychoactive medication and/or illicit drugs; or (4) had a history of concussion or traumatic brain injury in which they had lost consciousness for more than 10 min and/or presented to hospital. In addition, anxiety, depression, other psychopathologies and substance use disorders were screened for using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck et al., 1988), the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1996) and the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I. version 5.0; Sheehan et al., 1998), respectively. Participants were excluded if they scored 20 or above on the BDI (moderate to severe depression range), 16 or above on the BAI (moderate to severe anxiety range) or screened positive to any neuropsychiatric conditions in the M.I.N.I.
Meditator respondents were asked to describe their meditation practice. Participants were eligible to participate if their self-described practice adhered to Kabat-Zinn’s definition of mindfulness as "paying attention in a particular way: on purpose, in the present moment, and non-judgementally" (Kabat-Zinn, 1994, p. 4). Additional screening questions were asked to ensure the participants’ meditation practice involved focusing on sensations in the body (e.g. paying attention to the breath or body sensations). To address common methodological issues in meditation research of inadequate control for meditation type and level of experience (Van Dam et al., 2018), the current study employed the following criteria for what could be considered an experienced mindfulness meditation practitioner: Meditator participants, at the time of testing, had to be practicing mindfulness meditation (as per Kabat-Zinn’s definition) for 2 or more hours per week and must have been doing so for a minimum of 2 years. These criteria have been used in previous research (Bailey et al., 2019; Payne et al., 2019). Uncertainties regarding meditation experience and quality of practice, where there was not a clear and obvious match between participant’s practice and the inclusion criteria, were resolved through informal discussion with the participant to seek further details about their practice, and direct consultation with the Principal Investigator (NWB), who is an experienced meditation researcher. In uncertain cases, the final decision to include or exclude participants based on their meditation practice was reached by consensus between the Principal Investigator and one other researcher, after discussion of how closely the details of each potential participant’s practice matched the inclusion criteria, with cases that were still uncertain after discussion being excluded. Healthy controls were included in the non-meditator group as long as they had completed not more than 2 cumulative lifetime hours of any form of meditation practice and met all other inclusion criteria. Meditator participants were instructed to not meditate during the testing session. In addition, participants were instructed to avoid caffeine or alcohol for 24 hr prior to administration of the TMS-EEG protocol. At the start of the session, participants completed a TMS safety screen (Rossi et al., 2009, 2011) and a questionnaire that obtained demographic data (age, years of formal education, gender, handedness, descriptions of their meditation practice including average duration, length and frequency of meditation sessions and current medication use), after which participants completed the BAI, BDI-II and the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ). This was followed by verbal administration of the M.I.N.I. The current study was reviewed and approved by the Alfred Hospital and Monash University ethics committees, with informed written consent obtained for all participants in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Of the total 75 participants recruited, TMS-EEG data were not obtained for 37 participants. Of these 37 participants, data were not collected for the following reasons: 29 participants declined to receive TMS; 1 participant was excluded from receiving TMS due to safety concerns, as they reported a potential history of seizures; 2 were excluded due to scoring above the moderate to high anxiety range in the BAI; and for 5 participants, time restrictions precluded data collection. The high number of participants declining to receive TMS may have been due to the fact that participants were informed their data would still be useful to the other aspects of the study even if they declined to receive TMS, and that a high number of participants preferred not to receive brain stimulation when it was not clinically useful (although participant reasons for declining to participate in the TMS part of the study were not formally recorded, the research team recalls this as the primary anecdotally provided reason). For a further three participants, TMS-EEG data were excluded from analysis due to technical issues during recording. Data were excluded from analysis for a further single control participant due to reporting meditation experience above the 2-hr lifetime maximum limit (which was revealed after the testing session). The final sample of useable TMS-EEG data consisted of 19 controls aged between 20 and 57 years (10 females, Mage = 29.58 years, SDage = 11.48 years), and 15 meditators aged between 22 and 64 years (5 females, Mage = 35.67 years, SDage = 13.81 years).

Procedure

Each participant took part in a 45- to 60-min single-pulse TMS-EEG data collection block as part of a more extensive session involving EEG recordings and three computerised cognitive tasks. The average total session duration was approximately 3-and-a-half hours and involved the following behavioural tasks: (i) a Go/Nogo task (Hill et al., 2024), (ii) an auditory oddball task (Payne et al., 2019) and (iii) an attention blink task (Bailey et al., 2023a). Following these tasks, the TMS resting motor threshold (RMT) was obtained, then each participant received 105 single pulses of TMS to the left and right DLPFC (with the order counterbalanced between participants).
RMT was determined using electromyography (EMG). Three electrodes were placed surrounding the First Dorsal Interosseous (FDI) muscle on the contralateral (right) hand to the site of stimulation (left M1) and continuous electrical activity of the muscle was observed using Scope V3.7 (ADInstruments, NZ). TMS pulses were delivered to the motor cortex with increasing intensity until a muscle response was observed. RMT was determined as the intensity at which a 1-mV peak-to-peak amplitude motor-evoked potential was evoked in the FDI muscle in at least three out of five trials (Enticott et al., 2012; Rossini et al., 2015).
After RMT was obtained, participants were provided with 105 single pulses of TMS (delivered at a rate of one pulse every 4 s \(\pm\) 10% jitter to control for expectancy effects) to the left- and right-DLPFCs (L-DLPFC, R-DLPFC), respectively. Stimulation intensity was set at 110% of the RMT. Monophasic stimulation was applied using a Magstim 200 stimulator, which delivers monophasic pulses (Magstim Company Ltd., UK) with a figure-of-eight magnetic coil (wing diameter of 70 mm). This approach was employed as prior research has indicated that it evokes larger TEPs than biphasic stimulation (Casula et al., 2018), while evidence also suggests that monophasic and biphasic stimulation both produce similar spatiotemporal distributions of cortical responses (Biabani et al., 2019). Pulse delivery was automated using Signal version 3.08 software (Cambridge Electronic Design Ltd., UK) connected to a stimulator trigger box. The coil was placed tangentially on the head and positioned with the centre of the coil above frontal EEG electrodes, namely F3 and F4, in accordance with the international 10–20 system for anatomical positioning of TMS stimulation of targeted brain regions. Electrodes F3 and F4 were chosen based on their spatial proximity to the L-DLPFC and R-DLPFC, respectively (Herwig et al., 2003). The TMS coil handle was pointed towards the rear of the participant and 45 degrees laterally from the mid-sagittal line (in accordance with current conventions; Enticott et al., 2012; Rossini et al., 2015). To control for auditory-evoked potentials due to loud clicking noises associated with delivery of each pulse, white noise was delivered using Neuroscan 10Ω ¼ stereo intra-auricular earphone inserts (Compumedics, Melbourne, Australia). The amplitude of white noise was increased gradually to a level that was loud enough to mask the click noises but still comfortable for the participant. Prior to administration of the TMS protocol, participants were asked to remain awake and to keep head movements to a minimum.

Measures

The FFMQ has been shown to provide reliable measures of overall trait mindfulness (McDonald’s ω > 0.89) (Lecuona et al., 2020). Cortical electrical activity was obtained using a 64 channel Compumedics Neuroscan Quik-Cap (Compumedics Ltd., Australia) with Ag/AgCl sintered electrodes in the standard 10–20 spatial format. The online reference electrode was located along the midline between CPz and Cz. Additional electrooculography recordings were obtained using a single supraorbital electrode positioned above the left eye in line with the pupil. ECI-Electrogel (Electro-Cap International, Inc., USA) was applied to each electrode and impedances were kept below 5kΩ throughout the duration of the session. EEG data were recorded using DC-coupled 1000 × amplification of the EEG signal, acquired using a SynAmps2 amplifier (SynAmps2, EDIT Compumedics Neuroscan, Texas, USA) and recorded using Neuroscan Acquire software V4.5 (Compumedics Ltd., Australia). As per standard TMS-EEG recording guidelines, EEG was recorded using a high sampling rate of 10 kHz (for review, see Farzan et al. (2016)). Amplifier saturation during TMS delivery was mitigated by using an operating range of ± 200 mV with a DC-2 kHz low pass filter applied.
EEG data were processed and analysed offline in Matlab R2018b (The MathWorks, USA) utilising the following toolboxes: EEGLAB (Delorme & Makeig, 2004), FieldTrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011), TESA (Rogasch et al., 2017) and Randomization Graphical User Interface (RAGU; Koenig et al., 2011). Prior to EEG data analysis, channels dedicated to muscle and eye artefact were removed. Initial processing involved removal of the large amplitude TMS pulse (− 5 to 15 ms), linear interpolation and the generation and concatenation of epochs (− 1 to 2 s around the TMS pulse). Data were downsampled to 1000 Hz and the removal of DC offset was achieved by baseline correcting each epoch to − 500 to − 50 ms prior to the TMS pulse. Large muscle artefacts, faulty channels and otherwise bad epochs were removed by visual inspection of the concatenated EEG trace (< 10% of total number of epochs selected for rejection). Two rounds of Independent Component Analysis (ICA; FastICA algorithm, "tanh" contrast function and semi-automated component) were implemented via the TESA toolbox in EEGLAB (Delorme & Makeig, 2004; Rogasch et al., 2017). The EEG signal was broken down into separate components via ICA. TESA then categorised each component as consisting of either neural or non-neural activity. The first round of ICA was implemented to identify and remove the large TMS pulse artefact and eye blinks. Once removed, data were bandpass filtered between 1 and 100 Hz (4th order Butterworth filter) and 50-Hz line noise was removed using a 50-Hz notch filter (band-stop filter between 48 and 52 Hz, 4th order Butterworth filter). Data were again visually inspected and bad epochs and channels were removed. A second round of ICA was performed using TESA’s semi-automated selection of neural and non-neural (eye blink/movement and muscle artefacts) components with visual inspection for conformation and rejection of artefactual activity. Missing channels were then interpolated using a spherical interpolation method (EEGLAB; pop_interp function). All channels were re-referenced to an average of all channels. All epochs were then collapsed into a single TEP by averaging each channel over all epochs for every individual and separately for both conditions (L-DLPFC and R-DLPFC). For further information on the TMS-EEG data analysis pipeline used in the current study, please see Rogasch et al. (2017). An example TMS-EEG analysis pipeline similar to the present study is provided at https://​nigelrogasch.​gitbooks.​io/​tesa-user-manual/​content/​example_​pipelines.​html (Rogasch, n.d.).
Average TEPs were generated for each subject and separately for each stimulated region of interest (L-DLPFC and R-DLPFC). Analysis focused on the a priori determined peaks of interest, namely P60 and N100, which have been suggested to measure excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission, respectively (Rogasch et al., 2017). To measure the P60 and N100 components, the average amplitude over pre-specified time windows was used (P60, 55–75 ms; N100, 90–130 ms) using automated scripts in Matlab. An analysis was also performed on the ratio of the P60 to the N100. Due to variable voltage shifts across the TEP epoch, some P60 values were negative when measured as averaged amplitude within the window of interest, a feature of the data which would result in non-sensible comparisons of the P60/N100 ratio if not addressed. To address this issue, we visually inspected the data to mark the peak P60 and N100 latencies. We then computed the difference between the peak P60 and N100 amplitude and the average of the minimum value within 50 ms of the TEP of interest. This provided positive values for all P60 deflections, and negative values for all N100 deflections, representing the size of the deflection compared to the surrounding ongoing TEP.

Data Analyses

To confirm adequate matching between control and meditator groups, the following statistical analyses were performed in SPSS (version 23): Independent samples t-tests were run to test for demographic group matching (age, years of formal education) and matching for scores on self-report measures (BAI, BDI and FFMQ). Chi-square tests were used to verify that groups did not differ in gender and handedness. A number of assumption checks and outlier checks were implemented prior to our statistical analyses. If the assumptions of a planned test were violated, we used a test that was robust to the assumption instead. Outliers were winsorised. The full details of these steps are reported in the Supplementary Information (assumption checks and outlier detection section).
To test whether the amplitudes of the P60 and N100 differed between groups, or whether group interacted with hemisphere (i.e. the site of stimulation), two separate mixed model ANOVAs were run in SPSS (version 23). The main effect of hemisphere was not included in our analysis plan, as a main effect of hemisphere did not offer valuable insight into the study’s primary focus, which aimed to determine the potential differences in neural activity associated with regular mindfulness meditation. Further analyses of the ratio of the P60 to the N100 (calculated by dividing each individual’s peak P60 deflection by their corresponding peak N100 deflection; \(\frac{\text{P}60}{\text{N}100})\) were performed using non-parametric Mann–Whitney U test statistics (Mann & Whitney, 1947). To control for multiple comparisons, the Benjamini-Hochberg False Discovery Rate (FDR) method was used (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995), and p-values adjusted for the FDR are reported as pFDR. Where null results were obtained, these were complimented by Bayes Factor (BF) analyses to quantify the probability of the null result. BF analyses determine the odds of observing the model describing the null hypothesis over that of the model describing the alternative hypothesis, expressed as an odds ratio with Bayes Factors > 1 favouring the null hypothesis model (Rouder et al., 2017).
To compliment the use of multivariate single electrode and time window analysis, the current study also performed statistical analysis using the Randomization Graphical User Interface software (RAGU; Koenig et al., 2011). The benefit of using RAGU to analyse EEG data is that it ameliorates several methodological and biasing issues by minimising the need for a priori selection of parameters (e.g. electrodes and time windows to analyse). In particular, RAGU uses non-parametric randomised permutation statistical techniques and whole EEG data (all channels, all epochs and all time points) to compare scalp field differences between groups at each time point along the averaged epoch. RAGU employs methods to control for multiple comparisons (Habermann et al., 2018; Koenig et al., 2011) by randomly permuting the data to generate distributions for which the null-hypothesis of no effect holds for each time point. The cumulative size of effects in the real data can then be compared to this distribution to obtain a ‘global count’ p-value, which is the probability of observing the real data at all timepoints in the epoch under the assumption that the null-hypothesis is true. This same process can also be applied to obtain a distribution of lengths of contiguous significant time periods in the randomised data termed ‘duration control’. Time periods of contiguous significance in the real data that exceed the 95% threshold in the distribution of the randomised data are then highlighted as effects with statistically significant durations in the real data. This duration control ensures the overall level of significance is maintained at the a priori determined level of significance (\(\alpha < 0.05\)).
To conduct the statistical tests, RAGU uses the Global Field Power (GFP) measure as a reference free measure of global neural response strength. GFP is equivalent to calculating the standard deviation from the average reference over all electrodes. The first step in the RAGU pipeline is to confirm that there exists a consistent activation of neural sources in response to TMS using the Topographical Consistency Test (TCT). This test ensures that significant results in subsequent RAGU analyses cannot be explained by extreme variation within a single group. The explanation of the TCT is provided in full in Supplementary Information.
Once there is confirmation of a consistent neural activation within groups and conditions using the TCT, further analysis is provided by two separate but related significance tests. The GFP test compared differences in overall neural response strength between groups (meditators and controls) and hemispheres (L-DLPFC and R-DLPFC stimulation) using the GFP as the measure of interest. The between-group/condition difference in the GFP of the real data is then compared to the null distribution of no effect; this distribution is generated by randomised shuffling of the individual observations 5000 times for each measured time point and drawing a distribution of the randomised GFP values. For each time point, the real data are compared to the randomised distribution and a p-value is determined. Values that exceed the 0.05 alpha-level threshold are considered significant, and the proportion of time points of the real data that exceed this threshold across the whole epoch determines the overall significance (i.e. the global count p-value). Contiguous periods of significance across the epoch are then controlled for using global duration statistics.
The GFP test measures differences between groups/conditions of neural response strength. To determine if the topographic distributions of active neural sources differ between groups, the topographical analysis of variance (TANOVA) test is performed. Importantly, this test does not require active sources to be localised to cortical regions to determine whether the topographical distributions differ. To achieve this, the TANOVA first applied the L2 normalisation of the data within groups/conditions by dividing the average group/condition voltage values by its scaling factor (the GFP). Any differences between groups/conditions can then be said to be independent of the global strength of neural activity. The within-group electrode space data is then averaged, and the average electrode space data from one condition/group is subtracted from the average electrode space data from another condition/group. The dGFP is then calculated from this between group/condition normalised difference data. If there are true between-group differences in active brain sources generating the scalp field, then values for the dGFP will be large. Statistical analyses are provided by 5000 randomised permutations and the generation of a dGFP null-distribution of no effect. The global count p-value and duration control statistics are then derived in the same manner as the GFP test. We have reported our outcome statistics to three decimal places to allow sufficient precision to enable our effect sizes to be accurately included in meta-analysis.

Results

Between-Group Comparisons of Demographics and Scales

Most demographic variables were found to be normally distributed using Shapiro–Wilk’s test for normality and visual inspection of the histograms. However, scores for the BDI-II and BAI failed to meet normality assumptions (BDI-II: controlsW(19) = 0.836, p = 0.004, skewness = 0.854 (SE = 0.524), kurtosis = − 0.649 (SE = 1.014); meditatorsW(15) = 0.259, p = 0.006, skewness = 0.976 (SE = 0.580), kurtosis = − 0.096 (SE = 1.121); BAI: meditatorsW(15) = 0.832, p = 0.010 skewness = 1.347 (SE = 0.580), kurtosis = − 0.096 (SE = 1.121)). To mitigate the effect of these violations, comparisons between these data were conducted using non-parametric Mann–Whitney U tests, and they were found to be non-significant (BDI-II: U = 110, p = 0.271; BAI: U = 128, p = 0.817). No significant between-group differences were observed in age, gender, years of education or handedness using independent samples t-tests (all p-values ≥ 0.097; results provided in Table 1). However, a statistically significant difference was observed between groups on the FFMQ, with experienced meditators scoring an average 24.49 points higher compared to controls (t(32) = − 4.130, p < 0.001, 95% CI [− 36.57, − 12.41], d = 1.452). Demographic measures and results are presented in Table 1.
Table 1
Reports of demographic variables, including measures of central tendency and statistics
 
Controls
M (SD)
Meditators
M (SD)
Statistics
Age
29.58 (11.48)
35.67 (13.81)
t(32) = 1.404, p = 0.170
Gender (male/female)
10/9
12/3
\(\chi\) 2(1) = 2.749, p = 0.097
Years of education
17.31 (2.04)
17.07 (2.71)
t(32) = 0.338, p = 0.737
Handedness (right/left/mixed)
15 / 3 / 1
15 / 0 / 0
\(\chi\) 2(2) = 3.579, p = 0.167
Years meditating
-
6.23 (4.55)
-
Hours spent meditating per week
-
7.49 (7.00)
-
Years of meditation experience
-
6.23 (2.97)
Minimum = 2,
Maximum = 12
FFMQ
132.84 (19.12)
157.33 (14.26)
t(32) = 4.130, p < 0.001
BAI
5.06 (3.83)
5.80 (6.54)
U = 128.000, p = 0.817
BDI-II
4.32 (4.66)
2.40 (2.95)
U = 110.000, p = 0.271
SD standard deviation, FFMQ Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire, BAI Beck Anxiety Inventory, BDI Beck Depression Inventory, \(\chi\) 2 chi-square test statistic, t independent samples t-test statistic, U Mann–Whitney U statistic for non-parametric tests. All significance levels (\(\alpha\)) set at 0.05

Single Electrode Analyses of TMS-Evoked Potentials

The group-averaged single-electrode TEPs are provided in Fig. 1. Butterfly plots of the group averaged TEPs are depicted in Fig. 2, revealing characteristic TEP deflections at N45, P60, N100, P200 and N280.
To investigate whether P60 and N100 differed between groups, mixed model ANOVAs were employed using data obtained from the single electrodes of the stimulated sites (F3 and F4). Preliminary analysis of the separate mixed model ANOVAs of P60 and N100 amplitudes indicated that most assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance were met (Fmax, Box’s M, Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity). However, the assumption of between-group equality of error variance in P60 amplitudes was violated (Levene’s test for equality of error variances: L-DLPFC, F(1, 32) = 0.014, p < 0.05; R-DLPFC F(1, 32) = 0.231, p < 0.05). Unequal group sizes, as in the present study, further exacerbate deviations away from between-group equality of variance. Despite this violation, the mixed model ANOVA F-test has been shown to be robust against deviations from unequal group sizes if the disparity between them are small (largest group size/smallest group size < 1.5; current study 19/15 = 1.27; (Stevens, 2012)). Corrections for violations of the equality of error variances are limited with respect to mixed model ANOVAs. Applying a lower alpha threshold (\(\alpha\) < 0.05) is one suggested method to control for the inflation of a type-I error rate (Allen & Bennett, 2008). However, the current (and subsequent) ANOVA analyses show that no group or factor measures met the requirements for statistical significance, and methods for correcting for this would favour the results towards the null hypothesis of no effect. The interpretations of these null results without correction are therefore warranted.
No significant main effect of group in P60 amplitudes was observed (Group; F(1, 32) = 1.712, p = 0.200, \({\eta }^{2}\) = 0.051). In addition, no significant interaction effect between group and hemisphere was observed (Group × Hemisphere; F(1, 32) = 0.303, p = 0.586, \({\eta }^{2}\)= 0.003). No significant differences in N100 amplitudes were observed between groups (Group; F(1, 32) = 1.128, p = 0.296, \({\eta }^{2}\)= 0.034). In addition, no significant interaction effect between group and hemisphere was observed (Group × Hemisphere; F(1, 32) = 0.130, p = 0.721, \({\eta }^{2}\)= 0.001). The results of the ANOVA tests are displayed in Table 2.
Table 2
Mixed-model ANOVA results of the single electrode comparisons for between groups and sites of stimulation
Condition
Controls
M (SD)
Meditators
M (SD)
Statistics
P60 L-DLPFC (F3 electrode)
 − 0.118 (0.79)
0.580 (1.48)
Main effect of group: F(1, 32) = 1.712, p = 0.200 (pFDR = 0.504), \({\eta }_{p}^{2}\) = 0.051, \({\eta }^{2}\) = 0.051, BF01 = 1.768
Main effect of hemisphere: F(1, 32) = 0.303, p = 0.586 (pFDR = 0.704), \({\eta }_{p}^{2}\) = 0.009, \({\eta }^{2}\) = 0.004, BF01 = 3.620
Interaction: F(1, 32) = 0.256, p = 0.616 (pFDR = 0.704), \({\eta }_{p}^{2}\) = 0.008, \({\eta }^{2}\) = 0.003, BF01 = 6.306
P60 R-DLPFC (F4 electrode)
 − 0.133 (2.01)
0.228 (1.53)
N100 L-DLPFC (F3 electrode)
 − 0.791 (2.04)
 − 1.346 (1.88)
Main effect of group: F(1, 32) = 1.128, p = 0.296 (pFDR = 0.504), \({\eta }_{p}^{2}\) = 0.034, \({\eta }^{2}\) = 0.034, BF01 = 1.591
Main effect of hemisphere: F(1, 32) = 1.044, p = 0.315 (pFDR = 0.504), \({\eta }_{p}^{2}\) = 0.032, \({\eta }^{2}\) = 0.006, BF01 = 2.714
Interaction: F(1, 32) = 0.130, p = 0.721 (pFDR = 0.721), \({\eta }_{p}^{2}\) = 0.004, \({\eta }^{2}\) = 0.001, BF01 = 4.219
N100 R-DLPFC (F4 electrode)
 − 0.983 (2.18)
 − 1.747 (1.74)
M mean, SD standard deviation, FDR false discovery rate (displayed with the adjusted alpha level \(\alpha\) adj), \({\eta }_{p}^{2}\) partial eta squared, \({\eta }^{2}\) eta squared, BF01 Bayes factor analysis of the null hypothesis (BF01 > 1 favours the model of the null hypothesis). All significance alpha levels set at 0.05
Table 3
Mann–Whitney U test results for between-group analyses of the P60/N100 ratios using non-parametric analyses
Condition
Controls
M (SD)
Meditators
M (SD)
Non-parametric statistics
P60/N100 Ratio
L-DLPFC
(F3 electrode)
 − 0.672 (0.101)
 − 0.834 (0.107)
Mann–Whitney U test:
U = 270.5, p < 0.001*, d = 1.571 (pFDR = 0.004), BF10 = 50.670
P60/N100 Ratio
R-DLPFC
(F4 electrode)
 − 0.670 (0.098)
 − 0.806 (0.089)
Mann–Whitney U test:
U = 252.5, p < 0.001*, d = 1.447 (pFDR = 0.004), BF10 = 39.240
M mean, SD standard deviation, FDR false discovery rate (displayed with the adjusted alpha level \(\alpha\) adj), d Cohen’s measure of effect size determined using an independent samples t-test, BF10 Bayes factor analysis of the alternative hypothesis (BF10 > 1 favours the model of the alternative hypothesis). All alpha significance levels set at 0.05. Note that the tests remain significant after FDR correction. *reflects p-values that were significant at p < 0.001
The P60/N100 ratios violated the required assumption of normality in parametric repeated measures ANOVAs using the Shapiro–Wilk test (all p < 0.017). To control for the violation of these assumptions, Mann–Whitney U test statistics (Mann & Whitney, 1947) were used to test for differences in the P60/N100 ratios (results presented in Table 3). Significant between-group differences were observed in the L-DLPFC condition (U = 270.50, p < 0.001, d = 1.571, BF10 = 50.67, significant at FDR adjusted pFDR= 0.004) and R-DLPFC condition (U = 252.5, p < 0.001, d = 1.447, BF10 = 39.24, significant at FDR adjusted pFDR= 0.004). Meditators were observed to have higher P60/N100 ratios on average compared to controls, showing mean P60/N100 ratio values of approximately − 0.8 compared to the means for controls which were approximately − 0.67. Since P60/N100 ratio values of − 1 would indicate the P60 and N100 amplitudes were equal in size (but with opposite polarities), this result indicates that meditators P60 deflections were on average more similar in size to their N100 deflections compared to the control participants (who showed N100 deflections that were larger than their P60 deflections, see Fig. 3).
To assess the potential driver of this effect, we performed a post hoc exploration of the absolute difference between the P60 and N100 deflections. These results showed no significant between-group difference in the L-DLPFC condition (U = 108.00, p = 0.242) or R-DLPFC condition (U = 125.00, p = 0.560), indicating that while the relative difference between the P60 and N100 differs between the groups, the result is not driven by a between-group difference in the absolute difference between the P60 and N100. While our groups did not significantly differ in age or sex, they were not directly matched in these factors. As such, we repeated this analysis after randomly excluding four control participants (out of a pool of 12 control participants who fell into a younger age bracket, who, if excluded, would improve age matching between the groups) to improve the age and sex matching between the groups. This control analysis showed the same pattern and significant result as our primary analysis, with significant between-group differences still observed in the L-DLPFC condition (U = 210.50, p < 0.001) and R-DLPFC condition (U = 196.50, p < 0.001). Finally, to confirm differences in the P60/N100 ratios were not simply the result of differences in either the P60 or N100 when these TEPs were measured as deflections (rather than as averaged amplitudes within the window of interest as per our amplitude analyses), we performed two confirmatory repeated measures ANOVAs comparing the P60 and N100 deflection values that were used in computing the P60/N100 ratios. These analyses showed no significant differences between the groups or interactions between group and hemisphere in the P60 or N100 deflections (all p > 0.18). This result aligns with our null result for the P60 and N100 amplitudes when measured averaged across their windows of interest and indicates that the P60/N100 ratio effect is driven by between-group differences in the relationship between the P60 and N100 ratio.

Whole Scalp Field Analysis of TMS-Evoked Potentials

The TCT test showed that there was consistency in the active neural sources contributing to the scalp field throughout most of the epoch (− 100 to 50 ms) for both groups and conditions (see Supplementary Information, Fig. S2; overall p < 0.001). There were, however, short periods of inconsistent neural activity that were observed in all groups and conditions. Inconsistent activity can be observed in periods within the P60 time window used in the single electrode analysis (55–75 ms). Inconsistencies were also observed for meditators in the R-DLPFC condition between 280 and 287 ms, and for controls in the L-DLPFC condition between 312 and 347 ms. Inconsistencies in these periods indicate that significant results obtained in the TANOVA test (described in subsequent analyses) may be due to inconsistency in the topographical distribution of the neural response across individuals within a single group.
The between-group GFP tests for differences in neural response strength were non-significant (group main effect, global count p = 0.485, see Fig. 4). Additionally, no significant interaction between group and condition was observed (hemisphere × group interaction, global count p = 0.610). Further, the brief periods of significance did not survive multiple comparison duration control (global duration statistics: main effect of group = 39 ms; hemisphere × group interaction = 30 ms). Of note, no periods of significance were observed around the time windows of interest in the hypothesis driven analyses of P60 and N100 in agreement with the single electrode analyses. Results of the GFP test indicate that there were no differences between groups in the overall strength of neural response to TMS to the DLPFC for either hemisphere.
In contrast to the null results in the GFP test, significant group differences in the distribution of active neural sources were observed in the TANOVA test lasting from 269 to 332 ms post TMS pulse (global count p = 0.016; Fig. 5a). This period was the only period to survive the multiple comparisons duration control of 32 ms (the main effect of group averaged significance and effect size between 269 and 332 ms were as follows: p = 0.008, \({\eta }^{2}\) = 0.126). While our groups did not significantly differ in age or sex, they were not directly matched in these factors. As such, we repeated this analysis after randomly excluding four control participants to improve the age and sex matching between the groups. This control analysis showed the same pattern and significant result as our primary analysis (p = 0.0176, \({\eta }^{2}\)= 0.119). No significant interaction between group and condition survived the multiple comparisons duration control of 25 ms (hemisphere × group interaction, global count p = 0.655). The significant group effect between 269 and 332 ms post TMS pulse is evidence that there were different spatial distributions of active neural sources contributing to the scalp field during this time. However, as topographical inconsistencies were observed in the TCT (meditators between 280 and 287 ms; controls between 312 and 347 ms), at least some of this effect may have been due to within group variation. Although there was no interaction effect, the TCT showed a different pattern of topographical consistency for stimulation to the different hemispheres. As such, we examined whether the significant main effect of group replicated within a single hemisphere showed consistent topographical activation. The results of this analysis are provided in Fig. 5c and d. No between-group significance was present in the L-DLPFC condition (global count p = 0.289), and no significant periods survived the multiple comparisons duration control of 31 ms (Fig. 5c). A significant between-group difference was observed within the R-DLPFC stimulation condition (global count p = 0.029) which lasted between 270 and 332 ms post TMS (\({\eta }^{2}\) = 0.097) and survived the multiple comparisons duration control of 31 ms. Inconsistencies observed in the TCT with R-DLPFC stimulation were only observed in meditators between 280 and 287 ms (Fig. 5d). The remaining time window (287–332 ms) still exceeded the multiple comparisons duration control of 31 ms. This result suggests that the meditation group showed a different topographical distribution of active neural sources following R-DLPFC stimulation. Further characterisation of the topographical distribution of neural activity over the period of significance is provided in Fig. 5.
The topographical distributions of neural activity averaged across 269 to 332 ms post TMS pulse showed less fronto-midline negativity and greater posterior negativity in meditators (Fig. 6a) when compared to controls (Fig. 6b). The meditator group also showed greater central and temporal positivity compared to controls. In the control group, the positive voltages appeared to be shifted more posteriorly, focused over centro-parietal regions. In addition, controls appeared to show more equivalent centro-parietal positivity across both hemispheres compared to meditators (who exhibited greater positive voltages in electrodes over the left hemisphere). The t-map (Fig. 6c) illustrates the difference (controls subtracted from meditators) in voltage distributions during the significant period. Specifically, the t-map indicates that the differences between groups appear to be strongest around the general region of the left-sided site of stimulation (F3, F5 and AF3 electrodes) with meditators showing more positive neural activity compared to controls. In addition, meditators showed more negative voltages compared to controls in parieto-occipital regions (PO3 and Pz electrodes).

Discussion

The current study aimed to use TMS-EEG measures of cortical reactivity to assess whether the DLPFC of individuals with an average of 6.23 years of meditation practice shows differences compared to demographically matched non-meditators. Our results indicated support for our hypothesis that meditators would differ in their P60/N100 ratio compared to matched controls, whereby meditators demonstrated significantly higher P60/N100 ratios, with strong Bayesian evidence despite the small sample size. Interestingly, contrary to our hypotheses, comparisons between the P60 and N100 components themselves did not significantly differ, nor did comparisons between the absolute difference between the P60 and N100. This null result for either TEP examined independently or the absolute difference between the TEPs, coupled with a larger P60/N100 ratio, may indicate that meditation is related to a difference in the balance between the two TEPs, but not a difference that is specific to only one of the TEPs, nor an effect that is influenced by the absolute difference between the TEPs (for example, when both TEPs are large but similar in size, the absolute difference provides a very large value, while the ratio provides a similar value to cases where both TEPs are small and similar in size). The larger P60/N100 ratio in meditators relative to healthy controls, in the absence of any significant difference in P60 or N100 alone, would typically be interpreted as a between-group difference in the E/I balance (Chung et al., 2018). However, we note that previous pharmacological research examining the relationship between the P60 or N100 and E/I has measured isolated TEP components rather than the ratio between the P60/N100, so the functional meaning of an altered P60/N100 ratio is not well established. Additionally, the meditation group showed a difference in the distribution of active neural sources 269–332 ms following DLPFC stimulation. Analysis of stimulation of L-DLPFC and R-DLPFC indicated that the period 270–332 ms post TMS remained significantly different between the groups and withstood multiple comparisons duration control with R-DLPFC stimulation. This late TEP activity has been suggested to reflect slow inhibitory post synaptic potentials or longer cortico-subcortical-cortical reverberation in response to TMS (Ferreri et al., 2011).
With regard to the interpretation of our results, the putative difference in the DLPFC’s E/I balance in experienced meditators could be explained with reference to the influence of broader differences in brain activity. In particular, a recently proposed model of the mechanisms underpinning the effects of focused attention mindfulness meditation suggests that the DLPFC and other executive control regions activate to over-ride default thought processes (Ganesan et al., 2022). Greater connectivity between the DLPFC and regions of the ventral and dorsal-corticolimbic networks (which are involved in top-down cognitive control processes) has been observed following a mindfulness meditation intervention (Taren et al., 2017). This top-down control is proposed to reduce the intensity of internal thought processes and allow focus to be sustained on interoceptive sensations (Ganesan et al., 2022). Recent evidence has highlighted the critical importance of E/I ratio in determining neural network dynamics that either improve or degrade local and interregional information transfer (Ma et al., 2019). As such, our current results may point to differences in E/I ratio in the DLPFC as a potential neural candidate underpinning meditation-related differences in top-down control by the DLPFC, which enable increased regulation of other brain regions, reduced mind wandering and improvements to cognitive processes (e.g. executive attention network functions). We note that previous research has identified a link between increased P60/N100 ratio and working memory performance, providing support for this suggestion (Chung et al., 2019). Recent evidence also suggests that the strongest difference in the resting brain activity of meditators is an increase in the temporal stability of the EEG signal in parietal regions of the cortex (a brain region associated with self-awareness, attention and somatosensory integration). This suggests the potential for an integrated view of meditation related changes to the brain, with increased regulation by the DLPFC causing more stable activity in other regions (Bailey et al., 2024). However, we note that our results do not indicate whether the E/I balance difference is specific to the DLPFC, or whether the difference might be present in other brain regions as well, potentially underpinning the aforementioned differences in neural activity that have been detected using alternative methods by other research. It may be that the repeated practice of mindfulness meditation is associated with neuroplastic changes in, for example, synaptic connectivity strength or neurotransmitter densities, which might underpin the P60/N100 differences, as well as the effects noted by previous research. However, this is speculative, so further longitudinal research is recommended to probe the potential pathway by which meditation practice might lead to altered P60/N100 ratios, as well as the suggested connectivity model of the effects of meditation on brain activity.
Several clinical disorders, including anxiety and depression, are believed to be associated with E/I ratio uncoupling and the resulting disordered prefrontal oscillatory phase coherence (Lisman, 2012; Radhu et al., 2015; Voineskos et al., 2019; Yizhar et al., 2011). The differences in E/I ratio following TMS applied to the DLPFC of meditators may suggest a potential target for therapeutic interventions, where therapies could aim to adjust the E/I ratio in a way that might contribute to the improvement of clinical symptoms following a mindfulness intervention (Piet & Hougaard, 2011; Strauss et al., 2014), Interestingly, recent research has suggested that a steeper N100 slope (driven by a larger P60 amplitude) is found in individuals who respond to TMS treatment for depression, a finding that may be analogous to our finding of a larger P60/N100 ratio in the current research, highlighting a potential clinical implication of the current study (Bailey et al., 2023c).
In contrast to the effects for the P60/N100 ratio, no differences were detected in the P60 and N100 peak amplitudes individually. As such, one possibility is that mindfulness meditation influences the processes which determine the balance between excitation and inhibition. However, with the present experimental design, we cannot rule out the alternative, namely that individuals with a pre-existing difference in E/I balance are prone to engage in meditation. It is worth noting that the generation of individual TEP components is not governed by functionally separate neurophysiological processes (Hill et al., 2016), and processes involved in altering the E/I ratio are also implicated in the generation of individual P60 and N100 amplitudes.
The 269–332-ms period of significance following DLPFC stimulation found in the mindful meditator group indicates differences in the neurophysiological processes leading to the formation of the N280 component with a peak latency falling between 266.7 \(\pm\) 32.2 ms (Ferreri et al., 2011) and 308.3 \(\pm\) 7.8 ms (Määttä et al., 2019). Polysynaptic circuits with potentials that are mediated via the generation of slow inhibitory post synaptic potential caused by GABAB/NMDA receptor–mediated neural transmission have been suggested to result in the long latency and wide cortical distributions (measured using EEG) of the N280 component (Farzan et al., 2013). These potentials may also arise from longer cortico-subcortical-cortical reverberation in response to TMS (Ferreri et al., 2011). Studies showed complete cessation of TEPs at around 150 ms post stimulation in unconscious individuals (Ferrarelli et al., 2010; Massimini et al., 2005; Rosanova et al., 2012; Sarasso et al., 2015), providing evidence that the higher-order prefrontal cognitive activity contribute to the N280 potential. However, previous characterisation of this component has primarily been in response to stimulation of M1 (Ferreri et al., 2011). Research has also suggested that a common neural mechanism exists between the generation of N100 and N280 (Farzan et al., 2013) due to correlation between the N100 and N280 and the length of the CSP (an indirect marker for GABABergic neurotransmission). However, previous research has only examined voltage amplitudes at single electrodes or clusters of electrodes with relevance to the N280. As such, it is not clear what the functional interpretation of differences in the distribution of activity for the N280 in experienced meditators reflects. Candidate explanations might be differences in neural connectivity which might affect the cortico-subcortical-cortical reverberatory response to the TMS pulse, differences in slow inhibitory processes, or other, as yet uncertain explanations.
Previous research has applied TMS stimulation to meditators after a 60-min meditation practice and showed enhanced GABABergic inhibitory neurotransmission (Guglietti et al., 2013). However, their findings may reflect a state effect rather than a trait effect (Tang et al., 2016). Additionally, the observed variations at the M1 site in meditators could be indicative of changes in somatosensory processes, such as self-related interoceptive sensory processing—known to be influenced by meditation (Tang, 2017). These variations are distinct from the attention and executive function-related brain regions that are more likely to be targeted by DLPFC stimulation in the present study. However, we note that since we only stimulated the DLPFC, we cannot comment on whether our results would be consistent across other brain regions or whether they are specific to the DLPFC. In particular, we note that the excitability of the motor cortex might be different to that of the DLPFC (Lioumis et al., 2009), and the same might be true of other brain regions. As such, our study provides valuable new insights by extending the application of TMS-EEG to the DLPFC in participants with extensive meditation experience.

Limitations and Future Directions

The functional interpretation of TEPs is controversial, with research suggesting that the P60 reflects excitatory synaptic activation and the N100 inhibitory processes, while other research suggests the TEPs are also influenced by auditory processing of the TMS click (Conde et al., 2019; ter Braack et al., 2015). The use of the sound masking protocol in the current study has been shown to reduce the influence of auditory processing on the N100, but residual influence is still suggested to remain (Biabani et al., 2019, 2021; Conde et al., 2019). While this controversy has implications for the interpretation of our results, it does not obviously invalidate our finding of differences in P60/N100 ratio, as it is not clear why auditory processing related effects would differ between meditators and non-meditators, and why any potential difference would influence the P60/N100 ratio. In particular, our previous research that examined neural activity in response to an auditory oddball using some of the same participants that were included in the current study indicated no differences between the meditator and non-meditator groups in auditory processing ERPs (Payne et al., 2019). Additionally, we are not aware of any existing data on the test–retest reliability of the P60/N100 ratio. As such, it is not clear whether the ratio would be consistent within an individual over time. However, given the consistency of the difference between meditators and non-meditators in the P60 to N100 ratio (Fig. 3), even if the measure varies to some degree between testing sessions, we suspect this random variation would be unlikely to provide an explanation for our results.
Another limitation is variability in the definition of the term mindfulness and the inclusion of heterogeneous samples of meditators from different traditions and with different levels of meditation experience are common methodological issues in meditation studies (Van Dam et al., 2018). These factors were controlled for in the current study to some extent by using an inclusion criteria that limited the meditation group to participants who practice breath/body focused meditation (Creswell, 2017; Norris et al., 2018; Tang, 2017; Tang et al., 2015). Additionally, some research has argued that distinctions between different mindfulness meditation practices may not provide much in the way of meaningful differences to neurophysiology, that the differences between meditators and non-meditators are likely to be considerably larger and that there is conceptual ambiguity in the definition of different meditation types and how these factors might affect neurophysiology (Bailey et al., 2023a; Schoenberg & Vago, 2019). Nonetheless, future research could reduce potential heterogeneity from the inclusion of a range of meditation practices by restricting recruitment of meditators to those who practice only a focused attention form of meditation and who have only practiced meditation for a specified number of years, for example.
Another common limitation of meditation research, including the current study, is the use of a cross-sectional study design, and the associated limitation in our ability to attribute causation in the differences associated with the meditator group. This limitation could be addressed by employing a controlled long-term prospective study. However, conducting such a study would be difficult given the effects observed in the present study are associated with an average of 6.23 years of practicing meditation. Nonetheless, structural and functional brain differences have been observed after mindfulness interventions ranging from 3 days to 8 weeks (Taren et al., 2017; Tomasino & Fabbro, 2016; Xue et al., 2011).
In addition to the cross-sectional nature of our study, sample selection factors might have affected our results. In particular, many participants of the broader study provided task-related EEG recordings but elected not to receive TMS. As such, our results may be affected by a self-selection bias. Similarly, although the two groups did not significantly differ in age, the meditator group’s mean age was older. This factor may have increased within group variability of the N100 amplitudes, and it is possible it may have influenced the null result for differences in N100 amplitudes. Future research could address this by directly age matching participants from each group.
Additionally, the TMS resting motor thresholds and single-pulse TMS-EEG measures were obtained after participants had completed a range of cognitive tasks. While it is conceivable that the completion of these tasks may have affected the RMT and/or TMS-EEG measures, both meditators and non-meditators completed the same tasks prior to the TMS measures. As such, this factor would only confound our conclusions if the effects of having completed cognitive tasks prior to the TMS measures were specific to only either the meditator or the non-meditator group, which we have no reason to believe is the case.
Finally, our study had a limited sample size. However, the use of Bayesian analyses indicated that our sample combined with the observed effect sizes provides moderate confidence in our conclusions for many of the null results, and strong confidence in our conclusions for the two positive results. However, future research with a larger sample size will provide more confidence and increase the potential generalisability of results.

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge the Dhamma Aloka Vipassana meditation centre in Melbourne and the Melbourne Insight Meditation centre for their assistance with the recruitment of several meditators who took part in the study.

Declarations

Conflict of Interest

In the last 3 years, PBF has received equipment for research from Neurosoft, Nexstim and Brainsway Ltd. He has served on scientific advisory boards for Magstim and LivaNova and received speaker fees from Otsuka. He has also acted as a founder and board member for TMS Clinics Australia and Resonance Therapeutics. PBF is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia Investigator grant (1193596). The other authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethics Information

Ethics approval was provided by the Ethics Committees of Monash University and Alfred Hospital. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation in the study.

Use of Artificial Intelligence

Artificial Intelligence was not used in the writing of this manuscript.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

BSL GOP_opleiding GZ-psycholoog

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
go back to reference Allen, P., & Bennett, K. (2008). SPSS for the health & behavioural sciences. Thomson. Allen, P., & Bennett, K. (2008). SPSS for the health & behavioural sciences. Thomson.
go back to reference Bailey, N. W., Freedman, G., Raj, K., Spierings, K. N., Piccoli, L. R., Sullivan, C. M., Chung, S. W., Hill, A. T., Rogasch, N. C., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2020). Mindfulness meditators show enhanced accuracy and different neural activity during working memory. Mindfulness, 11, 1762–1781. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01393-8CrossRef Bailey, N. W., Freedman, G., Raj, K., Spierings, K. N., Piccoli, L. R., Sullivan, C. M., Chung, S. W., Hill, A. T., Rogasch, N. C., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2020). Mindfulness meditators show enhanced accuracy and different neural activity during working memory. Mindfulness, 11, 1762–1781. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12671-020-01393-8CrossRef
go back to reference Bailey, N. W., Hoy, K. E., Sullivan, C. M., Allman, B., Rogasch, N. C., Daskalakis, Z. J., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2023c). Concurrent transcranial magnetic stimulation and electroencephalography measures are associated with antidepressant response from rTMS treatment for depression. Journal of Affective Disorders Reports, 14, 100612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadr.2023.100612CrossRef Bailey, N. W., Hoy, K. E., Sullivan, C. M., Allman, B., Rogasch, N. C., Daskalakis, Z. J., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2023c). Concurrent transcranial magnetic stimulation and electroencephalography measures are associated with antidepressant response from rTMS treatment for depression. Journal of Affective Disorders Reports, 14, 100612. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​jadr.​2023.​100612CrossRef
go back to reference Biabani, M., Fornito, A., Coxon, J. P., Fulcher, B. D., & Rogasch, N. C. (2021). The correspondence between EMG and EEG measures of changes in cortical excitability following transcranial magnetic stimulation. The Journal of Physiology, 599(11), 2907–2932. https://doi.org/10.1113/JP280966CrossRefPubMed Biabani, M., Fornito, A., Coxon, J. P., Fulcher, B. D., & Rogasch, N. C. (2021). The correspondence between EMG and EEG measures of changes in cortical excitability following transcranial magnetic stimulation. The Journal of Physiology, 599(11), 2907–2932. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1113/​JP280966CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Britton, W. B., Davis, J. H., Loucks, E. B., Peterson, B., Cullen, B. H., Reuter, L., Rando, A., Rahrig, H., Lipsky, J., & Lindahl, J. R. (2018). Dismantling Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy: Creation and validation of 8-week focused attention and open monitoring interventions within a 3-armed randomized controlled trial. Behavior Research and Therapy, 101, 92–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.09.010CrossRef Britton, W. B., Davis, J. H., Loucks, E. B., Peterson, B., Cullen, B. H., Reuter, L., Rando, A., Rahrig, H., Lipsky, J., & Lindahl, J. R. (2018). Dismantling Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy: Creation and validation of 8-week focused attention and open monitoring interventions within a 3-armed randomized controlled trial. Behavior Research and Therapy, 101, 92–107. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​brat.​2017.​09.​010CrossRef
go back to reference Cash, R. F. H., Noda, Y., Zomorrodi, R., Radhu, N., Farzan, F., Rajji, T. K., Fitzgerald, P. B., Chen, R., Daskalakis, Z. J., & Blumberger, D. M. (2017). Characterization of glutamatergic and GABAA-mediated neurotransmission in motor and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex using paired-pulse TMS–EEG. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(2), 502–511. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2016.133CrossRefPubMed Cash, R. F. H., Noda, Y., Zomorrodi, R., Radhu, N., Farzan, F., Rajji, T. K., Fitzgerald, P. B., Chen, R., Daskalakis, Z. J., & Blumberger, D. M. (2017). Characterization of glutamatergic and GABAA-mediated neurotransmission in motor and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex using paired-pulse TMS–EEG. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(2), 502–511. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1038/​npp.​2016.​133CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chung, S. W., Rogasch, N. C., Hoy, K. E., Sullivan, C. M., Cash, R. F. H., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2018). Impact of different intensities of intermittent theta burst stimulation on the cortical properties during TMS-EEG and working memory performance. Human Brain Mapping, 39(2), 783–802. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23882CrossRefPubMed Chung, S. W., Rogasch, N. C., Hoy, K. E., Sullivan, C. M., Cash, R. F. H., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2018). Impact of different intensities of intermittent theta burst stimulation on the cortical properties during TMS-EEG and working memory performance. Human Brain Mapping, 39(2), 783–802. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​hbm.​23882CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chung, S. W., Sullivan, C. M., Rogasch, N. C., Hoy, K. E., Bailey, N. W., Cash, R. F. H., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2019). The effects of individualised intermittent theta burst stimulation in the prefrontal cortex: A TMS-EEG study. Human Brain Mapping, 40(2), 608–627. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24398CrossRefPubMed Chung, S. W., Sullivan, C. M., Rogasch, N. C., Hoy, K. E., Bailey, N. W., Cash, R. F. H., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2019). The effects of individualised intermittent theta burst stimulation in the prefrontal cortex: A TMS-EEG study. Human Brain Mapping, 40(2), 608–627. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​hbm.​24398CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hill, A. T., Chung, S. W., Emonson, M., Corcoran, A. W., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Bailey, N. W. (2024). Neural differences in conflict monitoring and stimulus expectancy processes in experienced meditators are likely driven by enhanced attention. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.12.23.630201 Hill, A. T., Chung, S. W., Emonson, M., Corcoran, A. W., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Bailey, N. W. (2024). Neural differences in conflict monitoring and stimulus expectancy processes in experienced meditators are likely driven by enhanced attention. bioRxiv. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1101/​2024.​12.​23.​630201
go back to reference Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday life (1st ed.). Hyperion. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday life (1st ed.). Hyperion.
go back to reference Mann, H. B., & Whitney, D. R. (1947). On a test of whether one of two random variables is stochastically larger than the other. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 18(1), 50–60.CrossRef Mann, H. B., & Whitney, D. R. (1947). On a test of whether one of two random variables is stochastically larger than the other. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 18(1), 50–60.CrossRef
go back to reference Payne, J. R., Baell, O., Geddes, H., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Emonson, M., Hill, A. T., Van Dam, N. T., Humble, G., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Bailey, N. W. (2019). Experienced meditators exhibit no differences to demographically-matched controls in theta phase synchronisation, P200, or P300 during an auditory oddball task. Mindfulness, 11, 643–659. https://doi.org/10.1101/608547CrossRef Payne, J. R., Baell, O., Geddes, H., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Emonson, M., Hill, A. T., Van Dam, N. T., Humble, G., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Bailey, N. W. (2019). Experienced meditators exhibit no differences to demographically-matched controls in theta phase synchronisation, P200, or P300 during an auditory oddball task. Mindfulness, 11, 643–659. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1101/​608547CrossRef
go back to reference Rossini, P. M., Burke, D., Chen, R., Cohen, L. G., Daskalakis, Z., Di Iorio, R., Di Lazzaro, V., Ferreri, F., Fitzgerald, P. B., George, M. S., Hallett, M., Lefaucheur, J. P., Langguth, B., Matsumoto, H., Miniussi, C., Nitsche, M. A., Pascual-Leone, A., Paulus, W., Rossi, S., & Ziemann, U. (2015). Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clinical Neurophysiology, 126(6), 1071–1107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2015.02.001CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rossini, P. M., Burke, D., Chen, R., Cohen, L. G., Daskalakis, Z., Di Iorio, R., Di Lazzaro, V., Ferreri, F., Fitzgerald, P. B., George, M. S., Hallett, M., Lefaucheur, J. P., Langguth, B., Matsumoto, H., Miniussi, C., Nitsche, M. A., Pascual-Leone, A., Paulus, W., Rossi, S., & Ziemann, U. (2015). Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clinical Neurophysiology, 126(6), 1071–1107. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​clinph.​2015.​02.​001CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Sarasso, S., Boly, M., Napolitani, M., Gosseries, O., Charland-Verville, V., Casarotto, S., Rosanova, M., Casali, A. G., Brichant, J. F., Boveroux, P., Rex, S., Tononi, G., Laureys, S., & Massimini, M. (2015). Consciousness and complexity during unresponsiveness induced by propofol, xenon, and ketamine. Current Biology, 25(23), 3099–3105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.014CrossRefPubMed Sarasso, S., Boly, M., Napolitani, M., Gosseries, O., Charland-Verville, V., Casarotto, S., Rosanova, M., Casali, A. G., Brichant, J. F., Boveroux, P., Rex, S., Tononi, G., Laureys, S., & Massimini, M. (2015). Consciousness and complexity during unresponsiveness induced by propofol, xenon, and ketamine. Current Biology, 25(23), 3099–3105. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​cub.​2015.​10.​014CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 22–33. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9881538.PubMed Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 22–33. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9881538.PubMed
go back to reference Stevens, J. P. (2012). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (5th ed.). Routledge. Stevens, J. P. (2012). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (5th ed.). Routledge.
go back to reference Van Dam, N. T., van Vugt, M. K., Vago, D. R., Schmalzl, L., Saron, C. D., Olendzki, A., Meissner, T., Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Gorchov, J., Fox, K. C. R., Field, B. A., Britton, W. B., Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., & Meyer, D. E. (2018). Mind the hype: A critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(1), 36–61. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617709589CrossRefPubMed Van Dam, N. T., van Vugt, M. K., Vago, D. R., Schmalzl, L., Saron, C. D., Olendzki, A., Meissner, T., Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Gorchov, J., Fox, K. C. R., Field, B. A., Britton, W. B., Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., & Meyer, D. E. (2018). Mind the hype: A critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(1), 36–61. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​1745691617709589​CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Voineskos, D., Blumberger, D. M., Zomorrodi, R., Rogasch, N. C., Farzan, F., Foussias, G., Rajji, T. K., & Daskalakis, Z. J. (2019). Altered transcranial magnetic stimulation-electroencephalographic markers of inhibition and excitation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 85(6), 477–486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.09.032CrossRefPubMed Voineskos, D., Blumberger, D. M., Zomorrodi, R., Rogasch, N. C., Farzan, F., Foussias, G., Rajji, T. K., & Daskalakis, Z. J. (2019). Altered transcranial magnetic stimulation-electroencephalographic markers of inhibition and excitation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 85(6), 477–486. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​biopsych.​2018.​09.​032CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wang, M. Y., Freedman, G., Raj, K., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Sullivan, C. M., Tan, W.-L., Van Dam, N. T., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Bailey, N. W. (2020). Mindfulness meditation alters neural activity underpinning working memory during tactile distraction. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20(6), 1216–1233. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-020-00828-yCrossRef Wang, M. Y., Freedman, G., Raj, K., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Sullivan, C. M., Tan, W.-L., Van Dam, N. T., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Bailey, N. W. (2020). Mindfulness meditation alters neural activity underpinning working memory during tactile distraction. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20(6), 1216–1233. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3758/​s13415-020-00828-yCrossRef
go back to reference Yizhar, O., Fenno, L. E., Prigge, M., Schneider, F., Davidson, T. J., O’Shea, D. J., Sohal, V. S., Goshen, I., Finkelstein, J., Paz, J. T., Stehfest, K., Fudim, R., Ramakrishnan, C., Huguenard, J. R., Hegemann, P., & Deisseroth, K. (2011). Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature, 477(7363), 171–178. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10360CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Yizhar, O., Fenno, L. E., Prigge, M., Schneider, F., Davidson, T. J., O’Shea, D. J., Sohal, V. S., Goshen, I., Finkelstein, J., Paz, J. T., Stehfest, K., Fudim, R., Ramakrishnan, C., Huguenard, J. R., Hegemann, P., & Deisseroth, K. (2011). Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature, 477(7363), 171–178. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1038/​nature10360CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metagegevens
Titel
TMS-EEG Shows Mindfulness Meditation Is Associated With a Different Excitation/Inhibition Balance in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex
Auteurs
Gregory Humble
Harry Geddes
Oliver Baell
Jake Elijah Payne
Aron T. Hill
Sung Wook Chung
Melanie Emonson
Melissa Osborn
Bridget Caldwell
Paul B. Fitzgerald
Robin Cash
Neil W. Bailey
Publicatiedatum
15-02-2025
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Mindfulness / Uitgave 2/2025
Print ISSN: 1868-8527
Elektronisch ISSN: 1868-8535
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-025-02519-6