Mindfulness meditation is a form of mental training that has been found to improve several aspects of general wellbeing (Gu et al.,
2015; Spijkerman et al.,
2016), including mental health outcomes (Braun et al.,
2019; Godfrin & van Heeringen,
2010). Previous research indicated that mindfulness meditation is associated with changes in brain structure and function (Fox et al.,
2014; Young et al.,
2018). These changes include reductions in the age-related decline of grey matter (Pagnoni & Cekic,
2007) as well as improvement in the performance of several cognitive processes (Chiesa & Malinowski,
2011; Tang et al.,
2015). In particular, processes related to attention and executive functions have been suggested to be improved by mindfulness meditation (Bailey et al.,
2020,
2023b; Wang et al.,
2020).
The interactions between different brain regions are crucial for brain function, but another fundamental aspect of neurophysiology is the relative balance between cortical excitatory and inhibitory inputs to cortical pyramidal cells (Zhou & Yu,
2018). At pyramidal cells, neurotransmitters and receptors work together within localised dendritic networks to facilitate the changes that lead to structural and functional modifications (Gulyaeva,
2017). Moreover, the balance of excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission at these neurons plays a key role in creating and maintaining stable oscillatory activity. This activity is not just essential for neuronal signalling, but also underpins higher-order cognitive functions and the brain’s neuroplastic responses to sensory stimuli (Meunier et al.,
2017). Therefore, the observed structural and functional changes in meditators might stem from heightened neuroplasticity arising from shifts in the balance of excitatory and inhibitory inputs to cortical circuits.
When EEG activity is measured after stimulating the DLPFC, reproducible TMS-evoked potentials (TEPs) can be measured. TEPs manifest as complex waveforms with distinct positive peaks and negative troughs at specific post-stimulation latencies. The P60 and N100 are two especially reproducible TEP components that are thought to be associated with glutamatergic excitatory neurotransmission and GABA
Bergic-mediated inhibitory neurotransmission respectively (Cash et al.,
2017; Noda et al.,
2017; Rogasch & Fitzgerald,
2013; Rogasch et al.,
2015). While evidence suggests that glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmissions are not the only factors affecting variations in the P60 and N100, a parsimonious interpretation posits that these components are influenced by both inhibitory and excitatory cortical neural activity, and that TEPs generally reflect cortical reactivity. Given the underlying neural basis for these components, several studies have used the P60 to N100 ratio as a marker of cortical E/I balance (Noda et al.,
2017; Voineskos et al.,
2019). However, existing research has primarily employed TMS-EEG to assess neural activity in meditators by providing stimulation at the primary motor cortex (M1) site. This research has indicated enhanced GABA
Bergic inhibitory neurotransmission in experienced meditators (Guglietti et al.,
2013). The stimulation in these investigations was applied immediately after a 60-min meditation session. As such, the observed effects may be state-dependent rather than trait-based, and therefore may not be informative about the long-term trait effects of meditation on the brain (Tang et al.,
2016). Further, the inhibitory transmission changes at M1 in meditators may reflect differences in somatosensory processes, such as changes in self-related interoceptive sensory processing (Tang,
2017), rather than attention- and executive function–related brain regions which are more likely probed by the DLPFC stimulation in the current study.
Considering the provided background, the study’s primary aim was to ascertain if N100 amplitudes differ between experienced mindfulness meditators and non-meditators. A secondary objective was to examine potential differences in the P60 between these two groups. The third aim was to determine if there were noticeable differences in the N100 to P60 ratio when comparing mindfulness meditators with non-meditators. We hypothesised that the N100 TEP component at the DLPFC would show increased amplitudes in experienced mindfulness meditators compared to matched controls reflecting increased inhibitory activity that might underpin the attention enhancements suggested to be associated with the practice of meditation. We also hypothesised that this would coincide with a compensatory increase of the P60 amplitude. While we did not expect that meditators would show a lateralisation effect, we stimulated both the left and right DLPFC to enable us to assess whether any effects were consistent across the different hemispheres of the brain. Furthermore, while we anticipated that meditators might show different E/I balances to non-meditators, we were not aware of research suggesting a specific direction for the potential difference. As such, we had a non-directional hypothesis that the E/I balance (operationally defined as the P60/N100 ratio) would differ in experienced mindfulness meditators when compared to matched controls. In addition to these targeted hypotheses, we conducted exploratory analyses on the EEG data to discern if the global neural response strength or the distribution of active neural sources varied between the two groups.
Discussion
The current study aimed to use TMS-EEG measures of cortical reactivity to assess whether the DLPFC of individuals with an average of 6.23 years of meditation practice shows differences compared to demographically matched non-meditators. Our results indicated support for our hypothesis that meditators would differ in their P60/N100 ratio compared to matched controls, whereby meditators demonstrated significantly higher P60/N100 ratios, with strong Bayesian evidence despite the small sample size. Interestingly, contrary to our hypotheses, comparisons between the P60 and N100 components themselves did not significantly differ, nor did comparisons between the absolute difference between the P60 and N100. This null result for either TEP examined independently or the absolute difference between the TEPs, coupled with a larger P60/N100 ratio, may indicate that meditation is related to a difference in the balance between the two TEPs, but not a difference that is specific to only one of the TEPs, nor an effect that is influenced by the absolute difference between the TEPs (for example, when both TEPs are large but similar in size, the absolute difference provides a very large value, while the ratio provides a similar value to cases where both TEPs are small and similar in size). The larger P60/N100 ratio in meditators relative to healthy controls, in the absence of any significant difference in P60 or N100 alone, would typically be interpreted as a between-group difference in the E/I balance (Chung et al.,
2018). However, we note that previous pharmacological research examining the relationship between the P60 or N100 and E/I has measured isolated TEP components rather than the ratio between the P60/N100, so the functional meaning of an altered P60/N100 ratio is not well established. Additionally, the meditation group showed a difference in the distribution of active neural sources 269–332 ms following DLPFC stimulation. Analysis of stimulation of L-DLPFC and R-DLPFC indicated that the period 270–332 ms post TMS remained significantly different between the groups and withstood multiple comparisons duration control with R-DLPFC stimulation. This late TEP activity has been suggested to reflect slow inhibitory post synaptic potentials or longer cortico-subcortical-cortical reverberation in response to TMS (Ferreri et al.,
2011).
With regard to the interpretation of our results, the putative difference in the DLPFC’s E/I balance in experienced meditators could be explained with reference to the influence of broader differences in brain activity. In particular, a recently proposed model of the mechanisms underpinning the effects of focused attention mindfulness meditation suggests that the DLPFC and other executive control regions activate to over-ride default thought processes (Ganesan et al.,
2022). Greater connectivity between the DLPFC and regions of the ventral and dorsal-corticolimbic networks (which are involved in top-down cognitive control processes) has been observed following a mindfulness meditation intervention (Taren et al.,
2017). This top-down control is proposed to reduce the intensity of internal thought processes and allow focus to be sustained on interoceptive sensations (Ganesan et al.,
2022). Recent evidence has highlighted the critical importance of E/I ratio in determining neural network dynamics that either improve or degrade local and interregional information transfer (Ma et al.,
2019). As such, our current results may point to differences in E/I ratio in the DLPFC as a potential neural candidate underpinning meditation-related differences in top-down control by the DLPFC, which enable increased regulation of other brain regions, reduced mind wandering and improvements to cognitive processes (e.g. executive attention network functions). We note that previous research has identified a link between increased P60/N100 ratio and working memory performance, providing support for this suggestion (Chung et al.,
2019). Recent evidence also suggests that the strongest difference in the resting brain activity of meditators is an increase in the temporal stability of the EEG signal in parietal regions of the cortex (a brain region associated with self-awareness, attention and somatosensory integration). This suggests the potential for an integrated view of meditation related changes to the brain, with increased regulation by the DLPFC causing more stable activity in other regions (Bailey et al.,
2024). However, we note that our results do not indicate whether the E/I balance difference is specific to the DLPFC, or whether the difference might be present in other brain regions as well, potentially underpinning the aforementioned differences in neural activity that have been detected using alternative methods by other research. It may be that the repeated practice of mindfulness meditation is associated with neuroplastic changes in, for example, synaptic connectivity strength or neurotransmitter densities, which might underpin the P60/N100 differences, as well as the effects noted by previous research. However, this is speculative, so further longitudinal research is recommended to probe the potential pathway by which meditation practice might lead to altered P60/N100 ratios, as well as the suggested connectivity model of the effects of meditation on brain activity.
Several clinical disorders, including anxiety and depression, are believed to be associated with E/I ratio uncoupling and the resulting disordered prefrontal oscillatory phase coherence (Lisman,
2012; Radhu et al.,
2015; Voineskos et al.,
2019; Yizhar et al.,
2011). The differences in E/I ratio following TMS applied to the DLPFC of meditators may suggest a potential target for therapeutic interventions, where therapies could aim to adjust the E/I ratio in a way that might contribute to the improvement of clinical symptoms following a mindfulness intervention (Piet & Hougaard,
2011; Strauss et al.,
2014), Interestingly, recent research has suggested that a steeper N100 slope (driven by a larger P60 amplitude) is found in individuals who respond to TMS treatment for depression, a finding that may be analogous to our finding of a larger P60/N100 ratio in the current research, highlighting a potential clinical implication of the current study (Bailey et al.,
2023c).
In contrast to the effects for the P60/N100 ratio, no differences were detected in the P60 and N100 peak amplitudes individually. As such, one possibility is that mindfulness meditation influences the processes which determine the balance between excitation and inhibition. However, with the present experimental design, we cannot rule out the alternative, namely that individuals with a pre-existing difference in E/I balance are prone to engage in meditation. It is worth noting that the generation of individual TEP components is not governed by functionally separate neurophysiological processes (Hill et al.,
2016), and processes involved in altering the E/I ratio are also implicated in the generation of individual P60 and N100 amplitudes.
The 269–332-ms period of significance following DLPFC stimulation found in the mindful meditator group indicates differences in the neurophysiological processes leading to the formation of the N280 component with a peak latency falling between 266.7
\(\pm\) 32.2 ms (Ferreri et al.,
2011) and 308.3
\(\pm\) 7.8 ms (Määttä et al.,
2019). Polysynaptic circuits with potentials that are mediated via the generation of slow inhibitory post synaptic potential caused by GABA
B/NMDA receptor–mediated neural transmission have been suggested to result in the long latency and wide cortical distributions (measured using EEG) of the N280 component (Farzan et al.,
2013). These potentials may also arise from longer cortico-subcortical-cortical reverberation in response to TMS (Ferreri et al.,
2011). Studies showed complete cessation of TEPs at around 150 ms post stimulation in unconscious individuals (Ferrarelli et al.,
2010; Massimini et al.,
2005; Rosanova et al.,
2012; Sarasso et al.,
2015), providing evidence that the higher-order prefrontal cognitive activity contribute to the N280 potential. However, previous characterisation of this component has primarily been in response to stimulation of M1 (Ferreri et al.,
2011). Research has also suggested that a common neural mechanism exists between the generation of N100 and N280 (Farzan et al.,
2013) due to correlation between the N100 and N280 and the length of the CSP (an indirect marker for GABA
Bergic neurotransmission). However, previous research has only examined voltage amplitudes at single electrodes or clusters of electrodes with relevance to the N280. As such, it is not clear what the functional interpretation of differences in the distribution of activity for the N280 in experienced meditators reflects. Candidate explanations might be differences in neural connectivity which might affect the cortico-subcortical-cortical reverberatory response to the TMS pulse, differences in slow inhibitory processes, or other, as yet uncertain explanations.
Previous research has applied TMS stimulation to meditators after a 60-min meditation practice and showed enhanced GABABergic inhibitory neurotransmission (Guglietti et al.,
2013). However, their findings may reflect a state effect rather than a trait effect (Tang et al.,
2016). Additionally, the observed variations at the M1 site in meditators could be indicative of changes in somatosensory processes, such as self-related interoceptive sensory processing—known to be influenced by meditation (Tang,
2017). These variations are distinct from the attention and executive function-related brain regions that are more likely to be targeted by DLPFC stimulation in the present study. However, we note that since we only stimulated the DLPFC, we cannot comment on whether our results would be consistent across other brain regions or whether they are specific to the DLPFC. In particular, we note that the excitability of the motor cortex might be different to that of the DLPFC (Lioumis et al.,
2009), and the same might be true of other brain regions. As such, our study provides valuable new insights by extending the application of TMS-EEG to the DLPFC in participants with extensive meditation experience.
Limitations and Future Directions
The functional interpretation of TEPs is controversial, with research suggesting that the P60 reflects excitatory synaptic activation and the N100 inhibitory processes, while other research suggests the TEPs are also influenced by auditory processing of the TMS click (Conde et al.,
2019; ter Braack et al.,
2015). The use of the sound masking protocol in the current study has been shown to reduce the influence of auditory processing on the N100, but residual influence is still suggested to remain (Biabani et al.,
2019,
2021; Conde et al.,
2019). While this controversy has implications for the interpretation of our results, it does not obviously invalidate our finding of differences in P60/N100 ratio, as it is not clear why auditory processing related effects would differ between meditators and non-meditators, and why any potential difference would influence the P60/N100 ratio. In particular, our previous research that examined neural activity in response to an auditory oddball using some of the same participants that were included in the current study indicated no differences between the meditator and non-meditator groups in auditory processing ERPs (Payne et al.,
2019). Additionally, we are not aware of any existing data on the test–retest reliability of the P60/N100 ratio. As such, it is not clear whether the ratio would be consistent within an individual over time. However, given the consistency of the difference between meditators and non-meditators in the P60 to N100 ratio (Fig.
3), even if the measure varies to some degree between testing sessions, we suspect this random variation would be unlikely to provide an explanation for our results.
Another limitation is variability in the definition of the term
mindfulness and the inclusion of heterogeneous samples of meditators from different traditions and with different levels of meditation experience are common methodological issues in meditation studies (Van Dam et al.,
2018). These factors were controlled for in the current study to some extent by using an inclusion criteria that limited the meditation group to participants who practice breath/body focused meditation (Creswell,
2017; Norris et al.,
2018; Tang,
2017; Tang et al.,
2015). Additionally, some research has argued that distinctions between different mindfulness meditation practices may not provide much in the way of meaningful differences to neurophysiology, that the differences between meditators and non-meditators are likely to be considerably larger and that there is conceptual ambiguity in the definition of different meditation types and how these factors might affect neurophysiology (Bailey et al.,
2023a; Schoenberg & Vago,
2019). Nonetheless, future research could reduce potential heterogeneity from the inclusion of a range of meditation practices by restricting recruitment of meditators to those who practice only a focused attention form of meditation and who have only practiced meditation for a specified number of years, for example.
Another common limitation of meditation research, including the current study, is the use of a cross-sectional study design, and the associated limitation in our ability to attribute causation in the differences associated with the meditator group. This limitation could be addressed by employing a controlled long-term prospective study. However, conducting such a study would be difficult given the effects observed in the present study are associated with an average of 6.23 years of practicing meditation. Nonetheless, structural and functional brain differences have been observed after mindfulness interventions ranging from 3 days to 8 weeks (Taren et al.,
2017; Tomasino & Fabbro,
2016; Xue et al.,
2011).
In addition to the cross-sectional nature of our study, sample selection factors might have affected our results. In particular, many participants of the broader study provided task-related EEG recordings but elected not to receive TMS. As such, our results may be affected by a self-selection bias. Similarly, although the two groups did not significantly differ in age, the meditator group’s mean age was older. This factor may have increased within group variability of the N100 amplitudes, and it is possible it may have influenced the null result for differences in N100 amplitudes. Future research could address this by directly age matching participants from each group.
Additionally, the TMS resting motor thresholds and single-pulse TMS-EEG measures were obtained after participants had completed a range of cognitive tasks. While it is conceivable that the completion of these tasks may have affected the RMT and/or TMS-EEG measures, both meditators and non-meditators completed the same tasks prior to the TMS measures. As such, this factor would only confound our conclusions if the effects of having completed cognitive tasks prior to the TMS measures were specific to only either the meditator or the non-meditator group, which we have no reason to believe is the case.
Finally, our study had a limited sample size. However, the use of Bayesian analyses indicated that our sample combined with the observed effect sizes provides moderate confidence in our conclusions for many of the null results, and strong confidence in our conclusions for the two positive results. However, future research with a larger sample size will provide more confidence and increase the potential generalisability of results.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.